Loading…
Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better
Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic futu...
Saved in:
Published in: | Journal of Aging Research 2012, Vol.2012 (2012), p.248-257 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23 |
container_end_page | 257 |
container_issue | 2012 |
container_start_page | 248 |
container_title | Journal of Aging Research |
container_volume | 2012 |
creator | Sütterlin, Stefan Paap, Muirne C S Babic, Stana Kübler, Andrea Vögele, Claus |
description | Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1155/2012/267327 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_doaj_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_6996b58a907d4db1ae439b8c022ff2f7</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><airiti_id>P20151210007_201212_201704100006_201704100006_248_257</airiti_id><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_6996b58a907d4db1ae439b8c022ff2f7</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>1000405478</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU1v1DAQhiMEolXpiTMoR1S0dDy2Y4cDUltBqbQSCMrZcuLJ1qskLk5SxL_Hacqq7aW--GMePfb4zbLXDD4wJuUxAsNjLBRH9SzbRyhhhcjw-b31XnY4DFtIg5cFFvAy20OUAIhqPzv6MXW-t6MPfW57l59s6GP-M3SUX175fjPk5zTmpzSOFF9lLxrbDnR4Nx9kv758vjz7ulp_O784O1mvrJRCrYigrhtQoAGc1VSKCl2jRY2clWlWUBOXaEFyhxUoK0GiaIC4LtAl6iC7WLwu2K25jr6z8a8J1pvbgxA3xsbR1y2ZoiyLSmpbgnLCVcyS4GWl69Rb02CjkuvT4rqeqo5cTf0YbftA-rDS-yuzCTeGc-BSsSR4dyeI4fdEw2g6P9TUtranMA2GpW8VkPrWCX2_oHUMwxCp2V3DwMxhmTkss4SV6Lf3X7Zj_0eTgKMFSDk4-8c_YXuzwJQQauwOFlJrVqT6eqlbH_3ozTZMsU8hmu_JIhnObahb4-wEpkDMR1A82ghtUCr-DzEBtoI</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1000405478</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better</title><source>Open Access: Wiley-Blackwell Open Access Journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Sütterlin, Stefan ; Paap, Muirne C S ; Babic, Stana ; Kübler, Andrea ; Vögele, Claus</creator><contributor>Hess, Thomas M.</contributor><creatorcontrib>Sütterlin, Stefan ; Paap, Muirne C S ; Babic, Stana ; Kübler, Andrea ; Vögele, Claus ; Hess, Thomas M.</creatorcontrib><description>Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2090-2212</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 2090-2204</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2090-2212</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1155/2012/267327</identifier><identifier>PMID: 22500227</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cairo, Egypt: Hindawi Limiteds</publisher><ispartof>Journal of Aging Research, 2012, Vol.2012 (2012), p.248-257</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 2012 Stefan Sütterlin et al.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2012 Stefan Sütterlin et al. 2012</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-4337-1296</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3303571/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3303571/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,4024,27923,27924,27925,53791,53793</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22500227$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Hess, Thomas M.</contributor><creatorcontrib>Sütterlin, Stefan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paap, Muirne C S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Babic, Stana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kübler, Andrea</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vögele, Claus</creatorcontrib><title>Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better</title><title>Journal of Aging Research</title><addtitle>J Aging Res</addtitle><description>Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study.</description><issn>2090-2212</issn><issn>2090-2204</issn><issn>2090-2212</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU1v1DAQhiMEolXpiTMoR1S0dDy2Y4cDUltBqbQSCMrZcuLJ1qskLk5SxL_Hacqq7aW--GMePfb4zbLXDD4wJuUxAsNjLBRH9SzbRyhhhcjw-b31XnY4DFtIg5cFFvAy20OUAIhqPzv6MXW-t6MPfW57l59s6GP-M3SUX175fjPk5zTmpzSOFF9lLxrbDnR4Nx9kv758vjz7ulp_O784O1mvrJRCrYigrhtQoAGc1VSKCl2jRY2clWlWUBOXaEFyhxUoK0GiaIC4LtAl6iC7WLwu2K25jr6z8a8J1pvbgxA3xsbR1y2ZoiyLSmpbgnLCVcyS4GWl69Rb02CjkuvT4rqeqo5cTf0YbftA-rDS-yuzCTeGc-BSsSR4dyeI4fdEw2g6P9TUtranMA2GpW8VkPrWCX2_oHUMwxCp2V3DwMxhmTkss4SV6Lf3X7Zj_0eTgKMFSDk4-8c_YXuzwJQQauwOFlJrVqT6eqlbH_3ozTZMsU8hmu_JIhnObahb4-wEpkDMR1A82ghtUCr-DzEBtoI</recordid><startdate>2012</startdate><enddate>2012</enddate><creator>Sütterlin, Stefan</creator><creator>Paap, Muirne C S</creator><creator>Babic, Stana</creator><creator>Kübler, Andrea</creator><creator>Vögele, Claus</creator><general>Hindawi Limiteds</general><general>Hindawi Puplishing Corporation</general><general>Hindawi Publishing Corporation</general><general>Hindawi Limited</general><scope>188</scope><scope>ADJCN</scope><scope>AHFXO</scope><scope>RHU</scope><scope>RHW</scope><scope>RHX</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4337-1296</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>2012</creationdate><title>Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better</title><author>Sütterlin, Stefan ; Paap, Muirne C S ; Babic, Stana ; Kübler, Andrea ; Vögele, Claus</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Sütterlin, Stefan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paap, Muirne C S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Babic, Stana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kübler, Andrea</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vögele, Claus</creatorcontrib><collection>Airiti Library</collection><collection>الدوريات العلمية والإحصائية - e-Marefa Academic and Statistical Periodicals</collection><collection>معرفة - المحتوى العربي الأكاديمي المتكامل - e-Marefa Academic Complete</collection><collection>Hindawi Publishing Complete</collection><collection>Hindawi Publishing Subscription Journals</collection><collection>Hindawi Publishing Open Access Journals</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>Journal of Aging Research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Sütterlin, Stefan</au><au>Paap, Muirne C S</au><au>Babic, Stana</au><au>Kübler, Andrea</au><au>Vögele, Claus</au><au>Hess, Thomas M.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better</atitle><jtitle>Journal of Aging Research</jtitle><addtitle>J Aging Res</addtitle><date>2012</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>2012</volume><issue>2012</issue><spage>248</spage><epage>257</epage><pages>248-257</pages><issn>2090-2212</issn><issn>2090-2204</issn><eissn>2090-2212</eissn><abstract>Rumination has been defined as a mode of responding to distress that involves passively focusing one's attention on symptoms of distress without taking action. This dysfunctional response style intensifies depressed mood, impairs interpersonal problem solving, and leads to more pessimistic future perspectives and less social support. As most of these results were obtained from younger people, it remains unclear how age affects ruminative thinking. Three hundred members of the general public ranging in age from 15 to 87 years were asked about their ruminative styles using the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ), depression and satisfaction with life. A Mokken Scale analysis confirmed the two-factor structure of the RSQ with brooding and reflective pondering as subcomponents of rumination. Older participants (63 years and older) reported less ruminative thinking than other age groups. Life satisfaction was associated with brooding and highest for the earlier and latest life stages investigated in this study.</abstract><cop>Cairo, Egypt</cop><pub>Hindawi Limiteds</pub><pmid>22500227</pmid><doi>10.1155/2012/267327</doi><tpages>10</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4337-1296</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 2090-2212 |
ispartof | Journal of Aging Research, 2012, Vol.2012 (2012), p.248-257 |
issn | 2090-2212 2090-2204 2090-2212 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_6996b58a907d4db1ae439b8c022ff2f7 |
source | Open Access: Wiley-Blackwell Open Access Journals; PubMed Central |
title | Rumination and Age: Some Things Get Better |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-07T05%3A18%3A25IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_doaj_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Rumination%20and%20Age:%20Some%20Things%20Get%20Better&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20Aging%20Research&rft.au=S%C3%BCtterlin,%20Stefan&rft.date=2012&rft.volume=2012&rft.issue=2012&rft.spage=248&rft.epage=257&rft.pages=248-257&rft.issn=2090-2212&rft.eissn=2090-2212&rft_id=info:doi/10.1155/2012/267327&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_doaj_%3E1000405478%3C/proquest_doaj_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a5547-ee0ccf070800da8e94b2df84c2319f8470ce352a053d2b07a50524f0e3862dc23%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1000405478&rft_id=info:pmid/22500227&rft_airiti_id=P20151210007_201212_201704100006_201704100006_248_257&rfr_iscdi=true |