Loading…

Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words

Background Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimul...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Brain and behavior 2023-06, Vol.13 (6), p.e3008-n/a
Main Authors: Klackl, Johannes, Blechert, Jens, Jonas, Eva
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4708-656b1ae82e763742a05d33af76c023cad1498ac1eb5bc2db0403166657fbaaa23
container_end_page n/a
container_issue 6
container_start_page e3008
container_title Brain and behavior
container_volume 13
creator Klackl, Johannes
Blechert, Jens
Jonas, Eva
description Background Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. Methods In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. Results Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. Conclusion The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. The manikin task requires overriding intuitive responses to approach and avoid valent stimuli. Avoiding vs. aproaching positive stimuli boosted the N450 and the conflict slow potential. We observed no conflict effect with negative words, presumably because negative valence is less associated with avoidance than positive valence is associated with approach.
doi_str_mv 10.1002/brb3.3008
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_doaj_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_735d73847f9a4c569c708041f6704f70</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_735d73847f9a4c569c708041f6704f70</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>2826393338</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4708-656b1ae82e763742a05d33af76c023cad1498ac1eb5bc2db0403166657fbaaa23</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kctqGzEUhkVpaEKSRV-gDHTTLpzorplVaUzTBgKBkq7F0WVsuePRVBrbZNdH6DPmSSrbaUgK0Ubi6OPj5_wIvSX4jGBMz00y7IxhXL9CR5RIOmFUNa-fvA_Rac4LXI4gnHL8Bh0yRaRQgh-h22ns2y7YsQp9BdUmJnf_-4-B7F0Fw5Ai2HkZwDoGB7311Qj5ZxVylccU-5lP1SaM82qIOYxh7XeCfIIOWuiyP324j9GPyy-302-T65uvV9PP1xPLFa4nUkhDwNfUK8kUp4CFYwxaJS2mzIIjvKnBEm-EsdQZzDEjUpbkrQEAyo7R1d7rIiz0kMIS0p2OEPRuENNMQxqD7bxWTDjFaq7aBrgVsrElAeaklQrzVuHi-rR3DSuz9M76fkzQPZM-_-nDXM_iWhNMlRCiKYYPD4YUf618HvUyZOu7DnofV1nTmlCBJRHb4O__Qxdxlfqyq0JRyRrGWF2oj3vKpphz8u1jGoL1tnu97V5vuy_su6fxH8l_TRfgfA9sQufvXjbpi-8XbKf8C7J3uQo</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2826393338</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words</title><source>Wiley Online Library Open Access</source><source>Publicly Available Content Database</source><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Klackl, Johannes ; Blechert, Jens ; Jonas, Eva</creator><creatorcontrib>Klackl, Johannes ; Blechert, Jens ; Jonas, Eva</creatorcontrib><description>Background Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. Methods In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. Results Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. Conclusion The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. The manikin task requires overriding intuitive responses to approach and avoid valent stimuli. Avoiding vs. aproaching positive stimuli boosted the N450 and the conflict slow potential. We observed no conflict effect with negative words, presumably because negative valence is less associated with avoidance than positive valence is associated with approach.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2162-3279</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2162-3279</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1002/brb3.3008</identifier><identifier>PMID: 37165754</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</publisher><subject>Anger ; approach ; avoidance ; Avoidance behavior ; Behavior ; Conflict, Psychological ; Emotions ; event‐related potential ; Evoked Potentials ; Food ; Humans ; manikin task ; Motivation ; Original ; Reaction Time</subject><ispartof>Brain and behavior, 2023-06, Vol.13 (6), p.e3008-n/a</ispartof><rights>2023 The Authors. published by Wiley Periodicals LLC.</rights><rights>2023 The Authors. Brain and Behavior published by Wiley Periodicals LLC.</rights><rights>2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4708-656b1ae82e763742a05d33af76c023cad1498ac1eb5bc2db0403166657fbaaa23</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-2619-4715</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2826393338/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2826393338?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,11562,25753,27924,27925,37012,37013,44590,46052,46476,53791,53793,75126</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37165754$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Klackl, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blechert, Jens</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jonas, Eva</creatorcontrib><title>Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words</title><title>Brain and behavior</title><addtitle>Brain Behav</addtitle><description>Background Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. Methods In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. Results Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. Conclusion The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. The manikin task requires overriding intuitive responses to approach and avoid valent stimuli. Avoiding vs. aproaching positive stimuli boosted the N450 and the conflict slow potential. We observed no conflict effect with negative words, presumably because negative valence is less associated with avoidance than positive valence is associated with approach.</description><subject>Anger</subject><subject>approach</subject><subject>avoidance</subject><subject>Avoidance behavior</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Conflict, Psychological</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>event‐related potential</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>manikin task</subject><subject>Motivation</subject><subject>Original</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><issn>2162-3279</issn><issn>2162-3279</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>24P</sourceid><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kctqGzEUhkVpaEKSRV-gDHTTLpzorplVaUzTBgKBkq7F0WVsuePRVBrbZNdH6DPmSSrbaUgK0Ubi6OPj5_wIvSX4jGBMz00y7IxhXL9CR5RIOmFUNa-fvA_Rac4LXI4gnHL8Bh0yRaRQgh-h22ns2y7YsQp9BdUmJnf_-4-B7F0Fw5Ai2HkZwDoGB7311Qj5ZxVylccU-5lP1SaM82qIOYxh7XeCfIIOWuiyP324j9GPyy-302-T65uvV9PP1xPLFa4nUkhDwNfUK8kUp4CFYwxaJS2mzIIjvKnBEm-EsdQZzDEjUpbkrQEAyo7R1d7rIiz0kMIS0p2OEPRuENNMQxqD7bxWTDjFaq7aBrgVsrElAeaklQrzVuHi-rR3DSuz9M76fkzQPZM-_-nDXM_iWhNMlRCiKYYPD4YUf618HvUyZOu7DnofV1nTmlCBJRHb4O__Qxdxlfqyq0JRyRrGWF2oj3vKpphz8u1jGoL1tnu97V5vuy_su6fxH8l_TRfgfA9sQufvXjbpi-8XbKf8C7J3uQo</recordid><startdate>202306</startdate><enddate>202306</enddate><creator>Klackl, Johannes</creator><creator>Blechert, Jens</creator><creator>Jonas, Eva</creator><general>John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</general><general>John Wiley and Sons Inc</general><general>Wiley</general><scope>24P</scope><scope>WIN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2619-4715</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202306</creationdate><title>Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words</title><author>Klackl, Johannes ; Blechert, Jens ; Jonas, Eva</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4708-656b1ae82e763742a05d33af76c023cad1498ac1eb5bc2db0403166657fbaaa23</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Anger</topic><topic>approach</topic><topic>avoidance</topic><topic>Avoidance behavior</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Conflict, Psychological</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>event‐related potential</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>manikin task</topic><topic>Motivation</topic><topic>Original</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Klackl, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blechert, Jens</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jonas, Eva</creatorcontrib><collection>Wiley Online Library Open Access</collection><collection>Wiley Online Library Free Content</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>Brain and behavior</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Klackl, Johannes</au><au>Blechert, Jens</au><au>Jonas, Eva</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words</atitle><jtitle>Brain and behavior</jtitle><addtitle>Brain Behav</addtitle><date>2023-06</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>13</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>e3008</spage><epage>n/a</epage><pages>e3008-n/a</pages><issn>2162-3279</issn><eissn>2162-3279</eissn><abstract>Background Valence and motivational direction are linked. We approach good things and avoid bad things, and experience overriding these links as conflicting. Positive valence is more consistently linked with approach than negative valence is linked with avoidance. Therefore, avoiding positive stimuli should produce greater behavioral and neural signs of conflict than approaching negative stimuli. Methods In the present event‐related potential study, we tested this assumption by contrasting positive and negative conflict. We used the manikin task, in which we read positive and negative words that they needed to approach and avoid. Results Consistent with our prediction, positive conflict prolonged reaction times more than negative conflict did. A late (500–1000 ms following word onset) event‐related potential that we identified as the Conflict slow potential, was only sensitive to positive conflict. Conclusion The results of this study support the notion that avoiding positive stimuli is more conflicting than approaching negative stimuli. The fact that the conflict slow potential is typically sensitive to response conflict rather than stimulus conflict suggests that the manikin task primarily requires people to override prepotent responses rather than to identify conflicting stimuli. Thus, the present findings also shed light on the psychological processes subserving conflict resolution in the manikin task. The manikin task requires overriding intuitive responses to approach and avoid valent stimuli. Avoiding vs. aproaching positive stimuli boosted the N450 and the conflict slow potential. We observed no conflict effect with negative words, presumably because negative valence is less associated with avoidance than positive valence is associated with approach.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</pub><pmid>37165754</pmid><doi>10.1002/brb3.3008</doi><tpages>10</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2619-4715</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 2162-3279
ispartof Brain and behavior, 2023-06, Vol.13 (6), p.e3008-n/a
issn 2162-3279
2162-3279
language eng
recordid cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_735d73847f9a4c569c708041f6704f70
source Wiley Online Library Open Access; Publicly Available Content Database; PubMed Central
subjects Anger
approach
avoidance
Avoidance behavior
Behavior
Conflict, Psychological
Emotions
event‐related potential
Evoked Potentials
Food
Humans
manikin task
Motivation
Original
Reaction Time
title Conflict in a word‐based approach‐avoidance task is stronger with positive words
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-28T02%3A13%3A23IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_doaj_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Conflict%20in%20a%20word%E2%80%90based%20approach%E2%80%90avoidance%20task%20is%20stronger%20with%20positive%20words&rft.jtitle=Brain%20and%20behavior&rft.au=Klackl,%20Johannes&rft.date=2023-06&rft.volume=13&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=e3008&rft.epage=n/a&rft.pages=e3008-n/a&rft.issn=2162-3279&rft.eissn=2162-3279&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002/brb3.3008&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_doaj_%3E2826393338%3C/proquest_doaj_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4708-656b1ae82e763742a05d33af76c023cad1498ac1eb5bc2db0403166657fbaaa23%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2826393338&rft_id=info:pmid/37165754&rfr_iscdi=true