Loading…
Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals
Animal alarm calls can contain detailed information about a predator’s threat, and heterospecific eavesdropping on these signals creates vast communication networks. While eavesdropping is common, this indirect public information is often less reliable than direct predator observations. Red-breasted...
Saved in:
Published in: | Nature communications 2020-01, Vol.11 (1), p.526-526, Article 526 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3 |
container_end_page | 526 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 526 |
container_title | Nature communications |
container_volume | 11 |
creator | Carlson, Nora V Greene, Erick Templeton, Christopher N |
description | Animal alarm calls can contain detailed information about a predator’s threat, and heterospecific eavesdropping on these signals creates vast communication networks. While eavesdropping is common, this indirect public information is often less reliable than direct predator observations. Red-breasted nuthatches (
Sitta canadensis
) eavesdrop on chickadee mobbing calls and vary their behaviour depending on the threat encoded in those calls. Whether nuthatches propagate this indirect information in their own calls remains unknown. Here we test whether nuthatches propagate direct (high and low threat raptor vocalizations) or indirect (high and low threat chickadee mobbing calls) information about predators differently. When receiving direct information, nuthatches vary their mobbing calls to reflect the predator’s threat. However, when nuthatches obtain indirect information, they produce calls with intermediate acoustic features, suggesting a more generic alarm signal. This suggests nuthatches are sensitive to the source and reliability of information and selectively propagate information in their own mobbing calls.
Animals can obtain information on predation risk directly from observing predators or indirectly from the alarm calls of others. Here, the authors show that red-breasted nuthatches encode information on risk in their own alarm calls differently depending on the source of the information. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1038/s41467-020-14414-w |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_doaj_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_737d9aa42f0841b1b96ecfa1dd5695a7</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_737d9aa42f0841b1b96ecfa1dd5695a7</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>2347514155</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9ksFu3CAQhlHVqonSvEAPlaVeenHKADZwqVRFaRMpag5Jz2gMeNcrr3HB3qhvX3adpkkP4TID880PMwwh74GeAeXqcxIgallSRksQ2S_vX5FjRgWUIBl__cQ_IqcpbWheXIMS4i054qCVYlIfk9sf87TGya59KnYYfxfT2nexwB7jtrDY96loMHlXzGMY9sEihTlaX4T2sPO488nFMI6ZSd1qwD69I2_abPzpgz0hP79d3J1fltc336_Ov16XthJ0KmULgAxZVbXIPTppBVCqmLa1ZryhgjrJdE0bLnSO65qB4rZxVkjLABw_IVeLrgu4MWPstrkAE7Azh4MQVwbj1NneG8ml04iCtVQJaKDRtbctgnNVrSuUWevLojXOzdY764cpYv9M9Hlk6NZmFXam1qoCQbPApweBGH7NPk1m2yXr-x4HH-ZkGBcyg1BVGf34H7rJPd13bk_VXGtFRabYQtkYUoq-fXwMULMfAbOMgMkjYA4jYO5z0oenZTym_P3wDPAFSDk0rHz8d_cLsn8AHqS8YQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2346399804</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals</title><source>Publicly Available Content Database</source><source>Nature</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Springer Nature - nature.com Journals - Fully Open Access</source><creator>Carlson, Nora V ; Greene, Erick ; Templeton, Christopher N</creator><creatorcontrib>Carlson, Nora V ; Greene, Erick ; Templeton, Christopher N</creatorcontrib><description>Animal alarm calls can contain detailed information about a predator’s threat, and heterospecific eavesdropping on these signals creates vast communication networks. While eavesdropping is common, this indirect public information is often less reliable than direct predator observations. Red-breasted nuthatches (
Sitta canadensis
) eavesdrop on chickadee mobbing calls and vary their behaviour depending on the threat encoded in those calls. Whether nuthatches propagate this indirect information in their own calls remains unknown. Here we test whether nuthatches propagate direct (high and low threat raptor vocalizations) or indirect (high and low threat chickadee mobbing calls) information about predators differently. When receiving direct information, nuthatches vary their mobbing calls to reflect the predator’s threat. However, when nuthatches obtain indirect information, they produce calls with intermediate acoustic features, suggesting a more generic alarm signal. This suggests nuthatches are sensitive to the source and reliability of information and selectively propagate information in their own mobbing calls.
Animals can obtain information on predation risk directly from observing predators or indirectly from the alarm calls of others. Here, the authors show that red-breasted nuthatches encode information on risk in their own alarm calls differently depending on the source of the information.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2041-1723</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2041-1723</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-14414-w</identifier><identifier>PMID: 31988279</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>London: Nature Publishing Group UK</publisher><subject>631/158/853 ; 631/158/856 ; 631/601/18 ; 64 ; Acoustic Stimulation ; Acoustics ; Alarm systems ; Animal Communication ; Animals ; Calling behavior ; Communication networks ; Cues ; Eavesdropping ; Humanities and Social Sciences ; Information processing ; Mobbing ; multidisciplinary ; Owls ; Passeriformes - physiology ; Predation ; Predators ; Predatory Behavior ; Public information ; Science ; Science (multidisciplinary) ; Sitta canadensis ; Vocalization behavior</subject><ispartof>Nature communications, 2020-01, Vol.11 (1), p.526-526, Article 526</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2020</rights><rights>This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-7785-1247 ; 0000-0002-8889-0209</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2346399804/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2346399804?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,25753,27924,27925,37012,37013,44590,53791,53793,74998</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31988279$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Carlson, Nora V</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Greene, Erick</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Templeton, Christopher N</creatorcontrib><title>Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals</title><title>Nature communications</title><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><description>Animal alarm calls can contain detailed information about a predator’s threat, and heterospecific eavesdropping on these signals creates vast communication networks. While eavesdropping is common, this indirect public information is often less reliable than direct predator observations. Red-breasted nuthatches (
Sitta canadensis
) eavesdrop on chickadee mobbing calls and vary their behaviour depending on the threat encoded in those calls. Whether nuthatches propagate this indirect information in their own calls remains unknown. Here we test whether nuthatches propagate direct (high and low threat raptor vocalizations) or indirect (high and low threat chickadee mobbing calls) information about predators differently. When receiving direct information, nuthatches vary their mobbing calls to reflect the predator’s threat. However, when nuthatches obtain indirect information, they produce calls with intermediate acoustic features, suggesting a more generic alarm signal. This suggests nuthatches are sensitive to the source and reliability of information and selectively propagate information in their own mobbing calls.
Animals can obtain information on predation risk directly from observing predators or indirectly from the alarm calls of others. Here, the authors show that red-breasted nuthatches encode information on risk in their own alarm calls differently depending on the source of the information.</description><subject>631/158/853</subject><subject>631/158/856</subject><subject>631/601/18</subject><subject>64</subject><subject>Acoustic Stimulation</subject><subject>Acoustics</subject><subject>Alarm systems</subject><subject>Animal Communication</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Calling behavior</subject><subject>Communication networks</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Eavesdropping</subject><subject>Humanities and Social Sciences</subject><subject>Information processing</subject><subject>Mobbing</subject><subject>multidisciplinary</subject><subject>Owls</subject><subject>Passeriformes - physiology</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Predatory Behavior</subject><subject>Public information</subject><subject>Science</subject><subject>Science (multidisciplinary)</subject><subject>Sitta canadensis</subject><subject>Vocalization behavior</subject><issn>2041-1723</issn><issn>2041-1723</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNp9ksFu3CAQhlHVqonSvEAPlaVeenHKADZwqVRFaRMpag5Jz2gMeNcrr3HB3qhvX3adpkkP4TID880PMwwh74GeAeXqcxIgallSRksQ2S_vX5FjRgWUIBl__cQ_IqcpbWheXIMS4i054qCVYlIfk9sf87TGya59KnYYfxfT2nexwB7jtrDY96loMHlXzGMY9sEihTlaX4T2sPO488nFMI6ZSd1qwD69I2_abPzpgz0hP79d3J1fltc336_Ov16XthJ0KmULgAxZVbXIPTppBVCqmLa1ZryhgjrJdE0bLnSO65qB4rZxVkjLABw_IVeLrgu4MWPstrkAE7Azh4MQVwbj1NneG8ml04iCtVQJaKDRtbctgnNVrSuUWevLojXOzdY764cpYv9M9Hlk6NZmFXam1qoCQbPApweBGH7NPk1m2yXr-x4HH-ZkGBcyg1BVGf34H7rJPd13bk_VXGtFRabYQtkYUoq-fXwMULMfAbOMgMkjYA4jYO5z0oenZTym_P3wDPAFSDk0rHz8d_cLsn8AHqS8YQ</recordid><startdate>20200127</startdate><enddate>20200127</enddate><creator>Carlson, Nora V</creator><creator>Greene, Erick</creator><creator>Templeton, Christopher N</creator><general>Nature Publishing Group UK</general><general>Nature Publishing Group</general><general>Nature Portfolio</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7ST</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7T7</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7TO</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>SOI</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7785-1247</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8889-0209</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200127</creationdate><title>Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals</title><author>Carlson, Nora V ; Greene, Erick ; Templeton, Christopher N</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>631/158/853</topic><topic>631/158/856</topic><topic>631/601/18</topic><topic>64</topic><topic>Acoustic Stimulation</topic><topic>Acoustics</topic><topic>Alarm systems</topic><topic>Animal Communication</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Calling behavior</topic><topic>Communication networks</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Eavesdropping</topic><topic>Humanities and Social Sciences</topic><topic>Information processing</topic><topic>Mobbing</topic><topic>multidisciplinary</topic><topic>Owls</topic><topic>Passeriformes - physiology</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Predatory Behavior</topic><topic>Public information</topic><topic>Science</topic><topic>Science (multidisciplinary)</topic><topic>Sitta canadensis</topic><topic>Vocalization behavior</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Carlson, Nora V</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Greene, Erick</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Templeton, Christopher N</creatorcontrib><collection>SpringerOpen</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Environment Abstracts</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Industrial and Applied Microbiology Abstracts (Microbiology A)</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>Environment Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>Nature communications</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Carlson, Nora V</au><au>Greene, Erick</au><au>Templeton, Christopher N</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals</atitle><jtitle>Nature communications</jtitle><stitle>Nat Commun</stitle><addtitle>Nat Commun</addtitle><date>2020-01-27</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>11</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>526</spage><epage>526</epage><pages>526-526</pages><artnum>526</artnum><issn>2041-1723</issn><eissn>2041-1723</eissn><abstract>Animal alarm calls can contain detailed information about a predator’s threat, and heterospecific eavesdropping on these signals creates vast communication networks. While eavesdropping is common, this indirect public information is often less reliable than direct predator observations. Red-breasted nuthatches (
Sitta canadensis
) eavesdrop on chickadee mobbing calls and vary their behaviour depending on the threat encoded in those calls. Whether nuthatches propagate this indirect information in their own calls remains unknown. Here we test whether nuthatches propagate direct (high and low threat raptor vocalizations) or indirect (high and low threat chickadee mobbing calls) information about predators differently. When receiving direct information, nuthatches vary their mobbing calls to reflect the predator’s threat. However, when nuthatches obtain indirect information, they produce calls with intermediate acoustic features, suggesting a more generic alarm signal. This suggests nuthatches are sensitive to the source and reliability of information and selectively propagate information in their own mobbing calls.
Animals can obtain information on predation risk directly from observing predators or indirectly from the alarm calls of others. Here, the authors show that red-breasted nuthatches encode information on risk in their own alarm calls differently depending on the source of the information.</abstract><cop>London</cop><pub>Nature Publishing Group UK</pub><pmid>31988279</pmid><doi>10.1038/s41467-020-14414-w</doi><tpages>1</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7785-1247</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8889-0209</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 2041-1723 |
ispartof | Nature communications, 2020-01, Vol.11 (1), p.526-526, Article 526 |
issn | 2041-1723 2041-1723 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_737d9aa42f0841b1b96ecfa1dd5695a7 |
source | Publicly Available Content Database; Nature; PubMed Central; Springer Nature - nature.com Journals - Fully Open Access |
subjects | 631/158/853 631/158/856 631/601/18 64 Acoustic Stimulation Acoustics Alarm systems Animal Communication Animals Calling behavior Communication networks Cues Eavesdropping Humanities and Social Sciences Information processing Mobbing multidisciplinary Owls Passeriformes - physiology Predation Predators Predatory Behavior Public information Science Science (multidisciplinary) Sitta canadensis Vocalization behavior |
title | Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T02%3A04%3A11IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_doaj_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Nuthatches%20vary%20their%20alarm%20calls%20based%20upon%20the%20source%20of%20the%20eavesdropped%20signals&rft.jtitle=Nature%20communications&rft.au=Carlson,%20Nora%20V&rft.date=2020-01-27&rft.volume=11&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=526&rft.epage=526&rft.pages=526-526&rft.artnum=526&rft.issn=2041-1723&rft.eissn=2041-1723&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038/s41467-020-14414-w&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_doaj_%3E2347514155%3C/proquest_doaj_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-7f11a2a255fa3ead7c4100829c6923b040d72960b3493ea962183cbdc47c211d3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2346399804&rft_id=info:pmid/31988279&rfr_iscdi=true |