Loading…

Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts

The eyes are never still during maintained gaze fixation. When microsaccades are not occurring, ocular position exhibits continuous slow changes, often referred to as drifts. Unlike microsaccades, drifts remain to be viewed as largely random eye movements. Here we found that ocular position drifts c...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:eLife 2020-08, Vol.9
Main Authors: Malevich, Tatiana, Buonocore, Antimo, Hafed, Ziad M
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843
container_end_page
container_issue
container_start_page
container_title eLife
container_volume 9
creator Malevich, Tatiana
Buonocore, Antimo
Hafed, Ziad M
description The eyes are never still during maintained gaze fixation. When microsaccades are not occurring, ocular position exhibits continuous slow changes, often referred to as drifts. Unlike microsaccades, drifts remain to be viewed as largely random eye movements. Here we found that ocular position drifts can, instead, be very systematically stimulus-driven, and with very short latencies. We used highly precise eye tracking in three well trained macaque monkeys and found that even fleeting (~8 ms duration) stimulus presentations can robustly trigger transient and stimulus-specific modulations of ocular position drifts, and with only approximately 60 ms latency. Such drift responses are binocular, and they are most effectively elicited with large stimuli of low spatial frequency. Intriguingly, the drift responses exhibit some image pattern selectivity, and they are not explained by convergence responses, pupil constrictions, head movements, or starting eye positions. Ocular position drifts have very rapid access to exogenous visual information.
doi_str_mv 10.7554/elife.57595
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_doaj_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_e32b1c900f804cdb805a9b9baee3813f</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A633135947</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_e32b1c900f804cdb805a9b9baee3813f</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A633135947</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNptkt1rFDEUxQdRbKl98l0GfFFktvmcZF4spVhdWChUBd9CJrlZs8xM1mSm6n_f7G6tHTF5yOXkd0_I5RTFS4wWgnN2Bp13sOCCN_xJcUwQRxWS7NvTR_VRcZrSBuUlmJS4eV4cUSK4pFweF-c3euttmUbfT92UKhv9LQxlH-zU6dGHoQyuTF34WQaTlVhuQ_J7PZNuTC-KZ053CU7vz5Pi69WHL5efqtX1x-XlxaoyNSNjBYwK04A2GCyypsEa1Q4ZbGtjGLGkhloKIyznhIJguMaoJVRgbqmgUjJ6UiwPvjbojdpG3-v4WwXt1V4Ica10HL3pQAElLTYNQk4iZmwrEddN27QagEpMXfZ6f_DaTm0P1sAwRt3NTOc3g_-u1uFWCcYIk3U2eHNvEMOPCdKoep8MdJ0eIExJEUaxxIQJlNHX_6CbMMUhj2pHSSm5oOQvtdb5A35wIb9rdqbqoqYUU94wkanFf6i8LfTehAGcz_qs4e2sITMj_BrXekpJLT_fzNl3B9bEkFIE9zAPjNQuagpWOWpqH7VMv3o8wgf2T7DoHaO3zQQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2438885732</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts</title><source>PubMed Central(OA)</source><source>ProQuest - Publicly Available Content Database</source><creator>Malevich, Tatiana ; Buonocore, Antimo ; Hafed, Ziad M</creator><creatorcontrib>Malevich, Tatiana ; Buonocore, Antimo ; Hafed, Ziad M</creatorcontrib><description>The eyes are never still during maintained gaze fixation. When microsaccades are not occurring, ocular position exhibits continuous slow changes, often referred to as drifts. Unlike microsaccades, drifts remain to be viewed as largely random eye movements. Here we found that ocular position drifts can, instead, be very systematically stimulus-driven, and with very short latencies. We used highly precise eye tracking in three well trained macaque monkeys and found that even fleeting (~8 ms duration) stimulus presentations can robustly trigger transient and stimulus-specific modulations of ocular position drifts, and with only approximately 60 ms latency. Such drift responses are binocular, and they are most effectively elicited with large stimuli of low spatial frequency. Intriguingly, the drift responses exhibit some image pattern selectivity, and they are not explained by convergence responses, pupil constrictions, head movements, or starting eye positions. Ocular position drifts have very rapid access to exogenous visual information.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2050-084X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2050-084X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.7554/elife.57595</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32758358</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: eLife Science Publications, Ltd</publisher><subject>Animals ; Binocular vision ; Confidence intervals ; Drift ; Eye ; Eye movements ; Fixation, Ocular ; fixational eye movements ; Frequency dependence ; Gaze ; Head Movements ; Latency ; Macaca mulatta ; microsaccades ; Neuroscience ; Observations ; Photoreceptors ; Reaction Time ; Short Report ; slow ocular position drifts ; Vision, Ocular ; Visual Perception</subject><ispartof>eLife, 2020-08, Vol.9</ispartof><rights>2020, Malevich et al.</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2020 eLife Science Publications, Ltd.</rights><rights>2020, Malevich et al. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2020, Malevich et al 2020 Malevich et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-3917-510X ; 0000-0003-3928-6248 ; 0000-0001-9968-119X</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2438885732/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2438885732?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,25753,27924,27925,37012,37013,44590,53791,53793,75126</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32758358$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Malevich, Tatiana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Buonocore, Antimo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hafed, Ziad M</creatorcontrib><title>Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts</title><title>eLife</title><addtitle>Elife</addtitle><description>The eyes are never still during maintained gaze fixation. When microsaccades are not occurring, ocular position exhibits continuous slow changes, often referred to as drifts. Unlike microsaccades, drifts remain to be viewed as largely random eye movements. Here we found that ocular position drifts can, instead, be very systematically stimulus-driven, and with very short latencies. We used highly precise eye tracking in three well trained macaque monkeys and found that even fleeting (~8 ms duration) stimulus presentations can robustly trigger transient and stimulus-specific modulations of ocular position drifts, and with only approximately 60 ms latency. Such drift responses are binocular, and they are most effectively elicited with large stimuli of low spatial frequency. Intriguingly, the drift responses exhibit some image pattern selectivity, and they are not explained by convergence responses, pupil constrictions, head movements, or starting eye positions. Ocular position drifts have very rapid access to exogenous visual information.</description><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Binocular vision</subject><subject>Confidence intervals</subject><subject>Drift</subject><subject>Eye</subject><subject>Eye movements</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular</subject><subject>fixational eye movements</subject><subject>Frequency dependence</subject><subject>Gaze</subject><subject>Head Movements</subject><subject>Latency</subject><subject>Macaca mulatta</subject><subject>microsaccades</subject><subject>Neuroscience</subject><subject>Observations</subject><subject>Photoreceptors</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Short Report</subject><subject>slow ocular position drifts</subject><subject>Vision, Ocular</subject><subject>Visual Perception</subject><issn>2050-084X</issn><issn>2050-084X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNptkt1rFDEUxQdRbKl98l0GfFFktvmcZF4spVhdWChUBd9CJrlZs8xM1mSm6n_f7G6tHTF5yOXkd0_I5RTFS4wWgnN2Bp13sOCCN_xJcUwQRxWS7NvTR_VRcZrSBuUlmJS4eV4cUSK4pFweF-c3euttmUbfT92UKhv9LQxlH-zU6dGHoQyuTF34WQaTlVhuQ_J7PZNuTC-KZ053CU7vz5Pi69WHL5efqtX1x-XlxaoyNSNjBYwK04A2GCyypsEa1Q4ZbGtjGLGkhloKIyznhIJguMaoJVRgbqmgUjJ6UiwPvjbojdpG3-v4WwXt1V4Ica10HL3pQAElLTYNQk4iZmwrEddN27QagEpMXfZ6f_DaTm0P1sAwRt3NTOc3g_-u1uFWCcYIk3U2eHNvEMOPCdKoep8MdJ0eIExJEUaxxIQJlNHX_6CbMMUhj2pHSSm5oOQvtdb5A35wIb9rdqbqoqYUU94wkanFf6i8LfTehAGcz_qs4e2sITMj_BrXekpJLT_fzNl3B9bEkFIE9zAPjNQuagpWOWpqH7VMv3o8wgf2T7DoHaO3zQQ</recordid><startdate>20200806</startdate><enddate>20200806</enddate><creator>Malevich, Tatiana</creator><creator>Buonocore, Antimo</creator><creator>Hafed, Ziad M</creator><general>eLife Science Publications, Ltd</general><general>eLife Sciences Publications Ltd</general><general>eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3917-510X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3928-6248</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9968-119X</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200806</creationdate><title>Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts</title><author>Malevich, Tatiana ; Buonocore, Antimo ; Hafed, Ziad M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Binocular vision</topic><topic>Confidence intervals</topic><topic>Drift</topic><topic>Eye</topic><topic>Eye movements</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular</topic><topic>fixational eye movements</topic><topic>Frequency dependence</topic><topic>Gaze</topic><topic>Head Movements</topic><topic>Latency</topic><topic>Macaca mulatta</topic><topic>microsaccades</topic><topic>Neuroscience</topic><topic>Observations</topic><topic>Photoreceptors</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Short Report</topic><topic>slow ocular position drifts</topic><topic>Vision, Ocular</topic><topic>Visual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Malevich, Tatiana</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Buonocore, Antimo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hafed, Ziad M</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Journals</collection><collection>ProQuest - Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>eLife</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Malevich, Tatiana</au><au>Buonocore, Antimo</au><au>Hafed, Ziad M</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts</atitle><jtitle>eLife</jtitle><addtitle>Elife</addtitle><date>2020-08-06</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>9</volume><issn>2050-084X</issn><eissn>2050-084X</eissn><abstract>The eyes are never still during maintained gaze fixation. When microsaccades are not occurring, ocular position exhibits continuous slow changes, often referred to as drifts. Unlike microsaccades, drifts remain to be viewed as largely random eye movements. Here we found that ocular position drifts can, instead, be very systematically stimulus-driven, and with very short latencies. We used highly precise eye tracking in three well trained macaque monkeys and found that even fleeting (~8 ms duration) stimulus presentations can robustly trigger transient and stimulus-specific modulations of ocular position drifts, and with only approximately 60 ms latency. Such drift responses are binocular, and they are most effectively elicited with large stimuli of low spatial frequency. Intriguingly, the drift responses exhibit some image pattern selectivity, and they are not explained by convergence responses, pupil constrictions, head movements, or starting eye positions. Ocular position drifts have very rapid access to exogenous visual information.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>eLife Science Publications, Ltd</pub><pmid>32758358</pmid><doi>10.7554/elife.57595</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3917-510X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3928-6248</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9968-119X</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 2050-084X
ispartof eLife, 2020-08, Vol.9
issn 2050-084X
2050-084X
language eng
recordid cdi_doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_e32b1c900f804cdb805a9b9baee3813f
source PubMed Central(OA); ProQuest - Publicly Available Content Database
subjects Animals
Binocular vision
Confidence intervals
Drift
Eye
Eye movements
Fixation, Ocular
fixational eye movements
Frequency dependence
Gaze
Head Movements
Latency
Macaca mulatta
microsaccades
Neuroscience
Observations
Photoreceptors
Reaction Time
Short Report
slow ocular position drifts
Vision, Ocular
Visual Perception
title Rapid stimulus-driven modulation of slow ocular position drifts
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T13%3A07%3A01IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_doaj_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Rapid%20stimulus-driven%20modulation%20of%20slow%20ocular%20position%20drifts&rft.jtitle=eLife&rft.au=Malevich,%20Tatiana&rft.date=2020-08-06&rft.volume=9&rft.issn=2050-084X&rft.eissn=2050-084X&rft_id=info:doi/10.7554/elife.57595&rft_dat=%3Cgale_doaj_%3EA633135947%3C/gale_doaj_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c642t-e437c9eac1ed0dc91a06f0c1d6cc42d26e687c7d5523e741610b23715d3738843%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2438885732&rft_id=info:pmid/32758358&rft_galeid=A633135947&rfr_iscdi=true