Loading…

Time, texts, and teaching in vocabulary acquisition: A rebuttal to Cobb (2016)

[...]it was precisely due to the weakness of these shallow instructional methods that rich vocabulary instruction was first promoted (Beck & McKeown, 1985; Stahl, 1986).5 Direct instruction of vocabulary also tends to produce word knowledge that is more fragile than that which is acquired incide...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Reading in a foreign language 2016-10, Vol.28 (2), p.307
Main Author: McQuillan, Jeff
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
Description
Summary:[...]it was precisely due to the weakness of these shallow instructional methods that rich vocabulary instruction was first promoted (Beck & McKeown, 1985; Stahl, 1986).5 Direct instruction of vocabulary also tends to produce word knowledge that is more fragile than that which is acquired incidentally through reading, with gains fading more quickly over time. [...]the results of at least one attempt at spaced repetitions with paired associate learning are not encouraging. According to Nations (2014) estimates, reading also yields about seven words per hour from the 3,000- to 8,000-word-family level (6,000 words/868 hours = 6.9 words/hour). Word retention estimates are based on the average percent gain for all three vocabulary measures (form recall, meaning recall, and meaning recognition) multiplied by the number of words tested. Since recall scores tend to drop more sharply than multiple-choice recognition scores from immediate to delayed posttest for instructed words versus those acquired in incidental conditions (see Rott, 2004), combining the three measures also biases the comparison in favor of the reading-plus words.
ISSN:1539-0578
1539-0578