Loading…
Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements
It was proposed to study the proprioceptive sensory coding of movement trajectories during the performance of two-dimensional "drawing-like" movements imposed on the tip of the foot. For this purpose, the activity of the muscle-spindle afferents from the Extensor digitorum longus, Tibialis...
Saved in:
Published in: | Experimental brain research 2000-10, Vol.134 (3), p.311-321 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-ee0cd059a16a4c87d72054f1695eda9e29af2a0d69e78952a1a486349ce1bfb33 |
---|---|
cites | |
container_end_page | 321 |
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 311 |
container_title | Experimental brain research |
container_volume | 134 |
creator | ROLL, Jean-Pierre BERGENHEIM, Mikael RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith |
description | It was proposed to study the proprioceptive sensory coding of movement trajectories during the performance of two-dimensional "drawing-like" movements imposed on the tip of the foot. For this purpose, the activity of the muscle-spindle afferents from the Extensor digitorum longus, Tibialis anterior, Extensor hallucis longus, and Peroneus lateralis muscles was recorded from the lateral peroneal nerve using the microneurographic technique. The drawing movements, describing geometrical shapes such as squares, triangles, ellipses, and circles, were imposed at a constant velocity in both the clockwise and counterclockwise directions. A total number of 44 muscle-spindle afferents were tested, 36 of which were identified as primary and eight as secondary afferents. Whatever the shape of the imposed foot movement, the primary endings from one muscle never discharged throughout the whole trajectory (on average, they discharged for only 49.2% of the length of the trajectory), whereas all the secondary endings discharged for most part of the drawing trajectories (average: 84.8%). The relationship between afferent discharge rate and direction could be described with a cosine-shaped tuning function. The peak of this function corresponded to the preferred sensory direction of the receptor-bearing muscles. The whole path of a given geometrical drawing movement was found to be coded in turn by each of the primary afferents originating from each of the muscles successively stretched. The contribution of each population of muscle afferents from each ankle muscle was represented by a "population vector", whose orientation was the preferred direction of the muscle under consideration and whose length was the mean instantaneous frequency of the afferent population. The "sum vector" corresponding to the sum of all these weighted "population vectors" was found to point in the instantaneous direction of the drawing trajectory, i.e., the tangent to the trajectory. These findings suggest that trajectory information is already encoded at the peripheral level on the basis of the integrated inputs provided by sets of receptors belonging to all the muscles acting on a given joint. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s002210000472 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_hal_p</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_01435186v1</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>17730869</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-ee0cd059a16a4c87d72054f1695eda9e29af2a0d69e78952a1a486349ce1bfb33</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUFv1DAQhS0EokvhyBVZQkLikDKOnTjmVlVAV1oEBzhHs_aEmjpxiJOt-AX8bRztqhUnTs9-_vQ048fYSwEXAkC_SwBlmU8ASpeP2EYoWRZCQP2YbQCEKlQjzBl7ltLP9So1PGVn-V1Vsqo37M_XKY6Tj5bG2R-Ij3FcAs4-DtxG54cfPHZ8vouF8z0NKfsYePD9nvfxQNmaE_cDv1l6HNJ7vt1e8M9LsoGKNPrBBeIdkdujveVumdY8N-Fd1iL4W3oIec6edBgSvTjpOfv-8cO3q-ti9-XT9upyV1ilmrkgAuugMihqVLbRTpdQqU7UpiKHhkqDXYngakO6MVWJAlVTS2UsiX23l_KcvT3m3mBo8-I9Tr_biL69vty1q7f-USWa-iAy--bIjlP8tVCa294nSyHgQHFJrS5lJRVU_wWF1hKa2mSwOIJ2iilN1N2PIKBd62z_qTPzr07By74n90Cf-svA6xOAyWLoJhysT_ecbrSBRv4Ft36oGA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>17730869</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements</title><source>Springer Link</source><creator>ROLL, Jean-Pierre ; BERGENHEIM, Mikael ; RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</creator><creatorcontrib>ROLL, Jean-Pierre ; BERGENHEIM, Mikael ; RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</creatorcontrib><description>It was proposed to study the proprioceptive sensory coding of movement trajectories during the performance of two-dimensional "drawing-like" movements imposed on the tip of the foot. For this purpose, the activity of the muscle-spindle afferents from the Extensor digitorum longus, Tibialis anterior, Extensor hallucis longus, and Peroneus lateralis muscles was recorded from the lateral peroneal nerve using the microneurographic technique. The drawing movements, describing geometrical shapes such as squares, triangles, ellipses, and circles, were imposed at a constant velocity in both the clockwise and counterclockwise directions. A total number of 44 muscle-spindle afferents were tested, 36 of which were identified as primary and eight as secondary afferents. Whatever the shape of the imposed foot movement, the primary endings from one muscle never discharged throughout the whole trajectory (on average, they discharged for only 49.2% of the length of the trajectory), whereas all the secondary endings discharged for most part of the drawing trajectories (average: 84.8%). The relationship between afferent discharge rate and direction could be described with a cosine-shaped tuning function. The peak of this function corresponded to the preferred sensory direction of the receptor-bearing muscles. The whole path of a given geometrical drawing movement was found to be coded in turn by each of the primary afferents originating from each of the muscles successively stretched. The contribution of each population of muscle afferents from each ankle muscle was represented by a "population vector", whose orientation was the preferred direction of the muscle under consideration and whose length was the mean instantaneous frequency of the afferent population. The "sum vector" corresponding to the sum of all these weighted "population vectors" was found to point in the instantaneous direction of the drawing trajectory, i.e., the tangent to the trajectory. These findings suggest that trajectory information is already encoded at the peripheral level on the basis of the integrated inputs provided by sets of receptors belonging to all the muscles acting on a given joint.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s002210000472</identifier><identifier>PMID: 11045356</identifier><identifier>CODEN: EXBRAP</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin: Springer</publisher><subject>Adult ; Biological and medical sciences ; Feedback ; Foot ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Handwriting ; Humans ; Leg - physiology ; Life Sciences ; Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration ; Movement - physiology ; Muscle Spindles - physiology ; Muscle, Skeletal - innervation ; Neurons and Cognition ; Neurons, Afferent - physiology ; Proprioception - physiology ; Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2000-10, Vol.134 (3), p.311-321</ispartof><rights>2001 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-ee0cd059a16a4c87d72054f1695eda9e29af2a0d69e78952a1a486349ce1bfb33</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=787908$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11045356$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.science/hal-01435186$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>ROLL, Jean-Pierre</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>BERGENHEIM, Mikael</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</creatorcontrib><title>Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>It was proposed to study the proprioceptive sensory coding of movement trajectories during the performance of two-dimensional "drawing-like" movements imposed on the tip of the foot. For this purpose, the activity of the muscle-spindle afferents from the Extensor digitorum longus, Tibialis anterior, Extensor hallucis longus, and Peroneus lateralis muscles was recorded from the lateral peroneal nerve using the microneurographic technique. The drawing movements, describing geometrical shapes such as squares, triangles, ellipses, and circles, were imposed at a constant velocity in both the clockwise and counterclockwise directions. A total number of 44 muscle-spindle afferents were tested, 36 of which were identified as primary and eight as secondary afferents. Whatever the shape of the imposed foot movement, the primary endings from one muscle never discharged throughout the whole trajectory (on average, they discharged for only 49.2% of the length of the trajectory), whereas all the secondary endings discharged for most part of the drawing trajectories (average: 84.8%). The relationship between afferent discharge rate and direction could be described with a cosine-shaped tuning function. The peak of this function corresponded to the preferred sensory direction of the receptor-bearing muscles. The whole path of a given geometrical drawing movement was found to be coded in turn by each of the primary afferents originating from each of the muscles successively stretched. The contribution of each population of muscle afferents from each ankle muscle was represented by a "population vector", whose orientation was the preferred direction of the muscle under consideration and whose length was the mean instantaneous frequency of the afferent population. The "sum vector" corresponding to the sum of all these weighted "population vectors" was found to point in the instantaneous direction of the drawing trajectory, i.e., the tangent to the trajectory. These findings suggest that trajectory information is already encoded at the peripheral level on the basis of the integrated inputs provided by sets of receptors belonging to all the muscles acting on a given joint.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Feedback</subject><subject>Foot</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Handwriting</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Leg - physiology</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration</subject><subject>Movement - physiology</subject><subject>Muscle Spindles - physiology</subject><subject>Muscle, Skeletal - innervation</subject><subject>Neurons and Cognition</subject><subject>Neurons, Afferent - physiology</subject><subject>Proprioception - physiology</subject><subject>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2000</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkUFv1DAQhS0EokvhyBVZQkLikDKOnTjmVlVAV1oEBzhHs_aEmjpxiJOt-AX8bRztqhUnTs9-_vQ048fYSwEXAkC_SwBlmU8ASpeP2EYoWRZCQP2YbQCEKlQjzBl7ltLP9So1PGVn-V1Vsqo37M_XKY6Tj5bG2R-Ij3FcAs4-DtxG54cfPHZ8vouF8z0NKfsYePD9nvfxQNmaE_cDv1l6HNJ7vt1e8M9LsoGKNPrBBeIdkdujveVumdY8N-Fd1iL4W3oIec6edBgSvTjpOfv-8cO3q-ti9-XT9upyV1ilmrkgAuugMihqVLbRTpdQqU7UpiKHhkqDXYngakO6MVWJAlVTS2UsiX23l_KcvT3m3mBo8-I9Tr_biL69vty1q7f-USWa-iAy--bIjlP8tVCa294nSyHgQHFJrS5lJRVU_wWF1hKa2mSwOIJ2iilN1N2PIKBd62z_qTPzr07By74n90Cf-svA6xOAyWLoJhysT_ecbrSBRv4Ft36oGA</recordid><startdate>20001001</startdate><enddate>20001001</enddate><creator>ROLL, Jean-Pierre</creator><creator>BERGENHEIM, Mikael</creator><creator>RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</creator><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Verlag</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>1XC</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20001001</creationdate><title>Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements</title><author>ROLL, Jean-Pierre ; BERGENHEIM, Mikael ; RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-ee0cd059a16a4c87d72054f1695eda9e29af2a0d69e78952a1a486349ce1bfb33</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2000</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Feedback</topic><topic>Foot</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Handwriting</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Leg - physiology</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration</topic><topic>Movement - physiology</topic><topic>Muscle Spindles - physiology</topic><topic>Muscle, Skeletal - innervation</topic><topic>Neurons and Cognition</topic><topic>Neurons, Afferent - physiology</topic><topic>Proprioception - physiology</topic><topic>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>ROLL, Jean-Pierre</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>BERGENHEIM, Mikael</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>ROLL, Jean-Pierre</au><au>BERGENHEIM, Mikael</au><au>RIBOT-CISCAR, Edith</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2000-10-01</date><risdate>2000</risdate><volume>134</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>311</spage><epage>321</epage><pages>311-321</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><coden>EXBRAP</coden><abstract>It was proposed to study the proprioceptive sensory coding of movement trajectories during the performance of two-dimensional "drawing-like" movements imposed on the tip of the foot. For this purpose, the activity of the muscle-spindle afferents from the Extensor digitorum longus, Tibialis anterior, Extensor hallucis longus, and Peroneus lateralis muscles was recorded from the lateral peroneal nerve using the microneurographic technique. The drawing movements, describing geometrical shapes such as squares, triangles, ellipses, and circles, were imposed at a constant velocity in both the clockwise and counterclockwise directions. A total number of 44 muscle-spindle afferents were tested, 36 of which were identified as primary and eight as secondary afferents. Whatever the shape of the imposed foot movement, the primary endings from one muscle never discharged throughout the whole trajectory (on average, they discharged for only 49.2% of the length of the trajectory), whereas all the secondary endings discharged for most part of the drawing trajectories (average: 84.8%). The relationship between afferent discharge rate and direction could be described with a cosine-shaped tuning function. The peak of this function corresponded to the preferred sensory direction of the receptor-bearing muscles. The whole path of a given geometrical drawing movement was found to be coded in turn by each of the primary afferents originating from each of the muscles successively stretched. The contribution of each population of muscle afferents from each ankle muscle was represented by a "population vector", whose orientation was the preferred direction of the muscle under consideration and whose length was the mean instantaneous frequency of the afferent population. The "sum vector" corresponding to the sum of all these weighted "population vectors" was found to point in the instantaneous direction of the drawing trajectory, i.e., the tangent to the trajectory. These findings suggest that trajectory information is already encoded at the peripheral level on the basis of the integrated inputs provided by sets of receptors belonging to all the muscles acting on a given joint.</abstract><cop>Berlin</cop><pub>Springer</pub><pmid>11045356</pmid><doi>10.1007/s002210000472</doi><tpages>11</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0014-4819 |
ispartof | Experimental brain research, 2000-10, Vol.134 (3), p.311-321 |
issn | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_01435186v1 |
source | Springer Link |
subjects | Adult Biological and medical sciences Feedback Foot Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Handwriting Humans Leg - physiology Life Sciences Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration Movement - physiology Muscle Spindles - physiology Muscle, Skeletal - innervation Neurons and Cognition Neurons, Afferent - physiology Proprioception - physiology Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs |
title | Proprioceptive population coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans: II. Muscle-spindle feedback during drawing-like movements |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-07T17%3A56%3A54IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_hal_p&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Proprioceptive%20population%20coding%20of%20two-dimensional%20limb%20movements%20in%20humans:%20II.%20Muscle-spindle%20feedback%20during%20drawing-like%20movements&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=ROLL,%20Jean-Pierre&rft.date=2000-10-01&rft.volume=134&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=311&rft.epage=321&rft.pages=311-321&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.coden=EXBRAP&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s002210000472&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_hal_p%3E17730869%3C/proquest_hal_p%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-ee0cd059a16a4c87d72054f1695eda9e29af2a0d69e78952a1a486349ce1bfb33%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=17730869&rft_id=info:pmid/11045356&rfr_iscdi=true |