Loading…
Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making
For more than 60 years, it has been known that people report higher (lower) subjective values for items after having selected (rejected) them during a choice task. This phenomenon is coined "choice-induced preference change" or CIPC, and its established interpretation is that of "cogn...
Saved in:
Published in: | PloS one 2020-05, Vol.15 (5), p.e0231081-e0231081 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433 |
container_end_page | e0231081 |
container_issue | 5 |
container_start_page | e0231081 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 15 |
creator | Lee, Douglas Daunizeau, Jean |
description | For more than 60 years, it has been known that people report higher (lower) subjective values for items after having selected (rejected) them during a choice task. This phenomenon is coined "choice-induced preference change" or CIPC, and its established interpretation is that of "cognitive dissonance" theory. In brief, if people feel uneasy about their choice, they later convince themselves, albeit not always consciously, that the chosen (rejected) item was actually better (worse) than they had originally estimated. While this might make sense from an intuitive psychological standpoint, it is challenging from a theoretical evolutionary perspective. This is because such a cognitive mechanism might yield irrational biases, whose adaptive fitness would be unclear. In this work, we consider an alternative possibility, namely that CIPC is -at least partially- due to the refinement of option value representations that occurs while people are pondering about choice options. For example, contemplating competing possibilities during a choice may highlight aspects of the alternative options that were not considered before. In the context of difficult decisions, this would enable people to reassess option values until they reach a satisfactory level of confidence. This makes CIPC the epiphenomenal outcome of a cognitive process that is instrumental to the decision. Critically, our hypothesis implies novel predictions about how observed CIPC should relate to two specific meta-cognitive processes, namely: choice confidence and subjective certainty regarding pre-choice value judgments. We test these predictions in a behavioral experiment where participants rate the subjective value of food items both before and after choosing between equally valued items; we augment this traditional design with both reports of choice confidence and subjective certainty about value judgments. The results confirm our predictions and provide evidence that many quantitative features of CIPC (in particular: its relationship with metacognitive judgments) may be explained without ever invoking post-choice cognitive dissonance reduction explanation. We then discuss the relevance of our work in the context of the existing debate regarding the putative cognitive mechanisms underlying CIPC. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0231081 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2404324146</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A624289131</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_b2e6427c6a0d4f138c6b60f2b41c3b91</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A624289131</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNk9uO0zAQhiMEYpfCGyCIhITYixQfUifhAqmqgFaqtBKnW8txJomLE3djp8u-AM-NQ7OrZrUXKBeOJt_8f2Y8EwQvMZpjmuD3O9N3rdDzvWlhjgjFKMWPgnOcURIxgujjk_ez4Jm1O4QWNGXsaXBGSUwwy7Lz4M-qNsaqtgqva-HCawi1-gXhwQ7naVgOHHwI1-Z6eFcSItUWvYQi3HdQQgetHCjRVhA2qqpdKKTrhdY3YQ6haq3r-gZaJ3ToTFiAVFaZNmrEYPM8eFIKbeHFeM6CH58_fV-to-3ll81quY1kQpiLckhFKkVKY0yYKDOcJrkUCUoopIlMFnKRxUUs85QuFqyQJSGJLAuWxYxiiWNKZ8Hro-5eG8vHFlpOYhT7nmDPzYLNkSiM2PF9pxrR3XAjFP8XMF3FReeU1MBzAiz2DkygIi4xTSXLGSpJHmNJ8wx7rY-jW583UEhffSf0RHT6pVU1r8yBJ4RSf1de4OIoUN9LWy-3fIghkiYEIXYYzN6NZp256sE63igrQWvRgumPNTKaUTbIvrmHPtyJkaqEL1a1pfH_KAdRvmR-gFJf4WA7f4DyTwGNkn42S-Xjk4SLSYJnHPx2leit5ZtvX_-fvfw5Zd-esDUI7WprdO_8lNkpGB9B2Rlr_ezedRYjPqzWbTf4sFp8XC2f9ur0Mu-SbneJ_gUtwx4q</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2404324146</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making</title><source>Open Access: PubMed Central</source><source>Publicly Available Content Database</source><creator>Lee, Douglas ; Daunizeau, Jean</creator><contributor>Soltani, Alireza</contributor><creatorcontrib>Lee, Douglas ; Daunizeau, Jean ; Soltani, Alireza</creatorcontrib><description>For more than 60 years, it has been known that people report higher (lower) subjective values for items after having selected (rejected) them during a choice task. This phenomenon is coined "choice-induced preference change" or CIPC, and its established interpretation is that of "cognitive dissonance" theory. In brief, if people feel uneasy about their choice, they later convince themselves, albeit not always consciously, that the chosen (rejected) item was actually better (worse) than they had originally estimated. While this might make sense from an intuitive psychological standpoint, it is challenging from a theoretical evolutionary perspective. This is because such a cognitive mechanism might yield irrational biases, whose adaptive fitness would be unclear. In this work, we consider an alternative possibility, namely that CIPC is -at least partially- due to the refinement of option value representations that occurs while people are pondering about choice options. For example, contemplating competing possibilities during a choice may highlight aspects of the alternative options that were not considered before. In the context of difficult decisions, this would enable people to reassess option values until they reach a satisfactory level of confidence. This makes CIPC the epiphenomenal outcome of a cognitive process that is instrumental to the decision. Critically, our hypothesis implies novel predictions about how observed CIPC should relate to two specific meta-cognitive processes, namely: choice confidence and subjective certainty regarding pre-choice value judgments. We test these predictions in a behavioral experiment where participants rate the subjective value of food items both before and after choosing between equally valued items; we augment this traditional design with both reports of choice confidence and subjective certainty about value judgments. The results confirm our predictions and provide evidence that many quantitative features of CIPC (in particular: its relationship with metacognitive judgments) may be explained without ever invoking post-choice cognitive dissonance reduction explanation. We then discuss the relevance of our work in the context of the existing debate regarding the putative cognitive mechanisms underlying CIPC.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231081</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32421699</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Animal cognition ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Choice (Psychology) ; Choice Behavior - physiology ; Cognition ; Cognitive ability ; Cognitive Dissonance ; Confidence ; Context ; Decision making ; Decision Making - physiology ; Decision theory ; Emotions - physiology ; Female ; Food ; Food Preferences - psychology ; Humans ; Influence ; Judgments ; Life Sciences ; Male ; Medical imaging ; Metacognition ; Methods ; Middle Aged ; Neurons and Cognition ; Novels ; Personal preferences (Social sciences) ; Physical Sciences ; Predictions ; Preferences ; Prejudice ; Psychology and behavior ; Ratings & rankings ; Research and Analysis Methods ; Social Sciences ; Values ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2020-05, Vol.15 (5), p.e0231081-e0231081</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2020 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2020 Lee, Daunizeau. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><rights>2020 Lee, Daunizeau 2020 Lee, Daunizeau</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-5892-8694</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2404324146/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2404324146?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,25753,27924,27925,37012,37013,44590,53791,53793,75126</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32421699$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.sorbonne-universite.fr/hal-02872006$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Soltani, Alireza</contributor><creatorcontrib>Lee, Douglas</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Daunizeau, Jean</creatorcontrib><title>Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>For more than 60 years, it has been known that people report higher (lower) subjective values for items after having selected (rejected) them during a choice task. This phenomenon is coined "choice-induced preference change" or CIPC, and its established interpretation is that of "cognitive dissonance" theory. In brief, if people feel uneasy about their choice, they later convince themselves, albeit not always consciously, that the chosen (rejected) item was actually better (worse) than they had originally estimated. While this might make sense from an intuitive psychological standpoint, it is challenging from a theoretical evolutionary perspective. This is because such a cognitive mechanism might yield irrational biases, whose adaptive fitness would be unclear. In this work, we consider an alternative possibility, namely that CIPC is -at least partially- due to the refinement of option value representations that occurs while people are pondering about choice options. For example, contemplating competing possibilities during a choice may highlight aspects of the alternative options that were not considered before. In the context of difficult decisions, this would enable people to reassess option values until they reach a satisfactory level of confidence. This makes CIPC the epiphenomenal outcome of a cognitive process that is instrumental to the decision. Critically, our hypothesis implies novel predictions about how observed CIPC should relate to two specific meta-cognitive processes, namely: choice confidence and subjective certainty regarding pre-choice value judgments. We test these predictions in a behavioral experiment where participants rate the subjective value of food items both before and after choosing between equally valued items; we augment this traditional design with both reports of choice confidence and subjective certainty about value judgments. The results confirm our predictions and provide evidence that many quantitative features of CIPC (in particular: its relationship with metacognitive judgments) may be explained without ever invoking post-choice cognitive dissonance reduction explanation. We then discuss the relevance of our work in the context of the existing debate regarding the putative cognitive mechanisms underlying CIPC.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Animal cognition</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Choice (Psychology)</subject><subject>Choice Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Cognitive Dissonance</subject><subject>Confidence</subject><subject>Context</subject><subject>Decision making</subject><subject>Decision Making - physiology</subject><subject>Decision theory</subject><subject>Emotions - physiology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>Food Preferences - psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Influence</subject><subject>Judgments</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical imaging</subject><subject>Metacognition</subject><subject>Methods</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Neurons and Cognition</subject><subject>Novels</subject><subject>Personal preferences (Social sciences)</subject><subject>Physical Sciences</subject><subject>Predictions</subject><subject>Preferences</subject><subject>Prejudice</subject><subject>Psychology and behavior</subject><subject>Ratings & rankings</subject><subject>Research and Analysis Methods</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Values</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNk9uO0zAQhiMEYpfCGyCIhITYixQfUifhAqmqgFaqtBKnW8txJomLE3djp8u-AM-NQ7OrZrUXKBeOJt_8f2Y8EwQvMZpjmuD3O9N3rdDzvWlhjgjFKMWPgnOcURIxgujjk_ez4Jm1O4QWNGXsaXBGSUwwy7Lz4M-qNsaqtgqva-HCawi1-gXhwQ7naVgOHHwI1-Z6eFcSItUWvYQi3HdQQgetHCjRVhA2qqpdKKTrhdY3YQ6haq3r-gZaJ3ToTFiAVFaZNmrEYPM8eFIKbeHFeM6CH58_fV-to-3ll81quY1kQpiLckhFKkVKY0yYKDOcJrkUCUoopIlMFnKRxUUs85QuFqyQJSGJLAuWxYxiiWNKZ8Hro-5eG8vHFlpOYhT7nmDPzYLNkSiM2PF9pxrR3XAjFP8XMF3FReeU1MBzAiz2DkygIi4xTSXLGSpJHmNJ8wx7rY-jW583UEhffSf0RHT6pVU1r8yBJ4RSf1de4OIoUN9LWy-3fIghkiYEIXYYzN6NZp256sE63igrQWvRgumPNTKaUTbIvrmHPtyJkaqEL1a1pfH_KAdRvmR-gFJf4WA7f4DyTwGNkn42S-Xjk4SLSYJnHPx2leit5ZtvX_-fvfw5Zd-esDUI7WprdO_8lNkpGB9B2Rlr_ezedRYjPqzWbTf4sFp8XC2f9ur0Mu-SbneJ_gUtwx4q</recordid><startdate>20200518</startdate><enddate>20200518</enddate><creator>Lee, Douglas</creator><creator>Daunizeau, Jean</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5892-8694</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200518</creationdate><title>Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making</title><author>Lee, Douglas ; Daunizeau, Jean</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Animal cognition</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Choice (Psychology)</topic><topic>Choice Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Cognitive Dissonance</topic><topic>Confidence</topic><topic>Context</topic><topic>Decision making</topic><topic>Decision Making - physiology</topic><topic>Decision theory</topic><topic>Emotions - physiology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>Food Preferences - psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Influence</topic><topic>Judgments</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical imaging</topic><topic>Metacognition</topic><topic>Methods</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Neurons and Cognition</topic><topic>Novels</topic><topic>Personal preferences (Social sciences)</topic><topic>Physical Sciences</topic><topic>Predictions</topic><topic>Preferences</topic><topic>Prejudice</topic><topic>Psychology and behavior</topic><topic>Ratings & rankings</topic><topic>Research and Analysis Methods</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Values</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lee, Douglas</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Daunizeau, Jean</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Science (Gale in Context)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database (1962 - current)</collection><collection>Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Biological Sciences</collection><collection>Agriculture Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>ProQuest advanced technologies & aerospace journals</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials science collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>Open Access: DOAJ - Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lee, Douglas</au><au>Daunizeau, Jean</au><au>Soltani, Alireza</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2020-05-18</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>15</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>e0231081</spage><epage>e0231081</epage><pages>e0231081-e0231081</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>For more than 60 years, it has been known that people report higher (lower) subjective values for items after having selected (rejected) them during a choice task. This phenomenon is coined "choice-induced preference change" or CIPC, and its established interpretation is that of "cognitive dissonance" theory. In brief, if people feel uneasy about their choice, they later convince themselves, albeit not always consciously, that the chosen (rejected) item was actually better (worse) than they had originally estimated. While this might make sense from an intuitive psychological standpoint, it is challenging from a theoretical evolutionary perspective. This is because such a cognitive mechanism might yield irrational biases, whose adaptive fitness would be unclear. In this work, we consider an alternative possibility, namely that CIPC is -at least partially- due to the refinement of option value representations that occurs while people are pondering about choice options. For example, contemplating competing possibilities during a choice may highlight aspects of the alternative options that were not considered before. In the context of difficult decisions, this would enable people to reassess option values until they reach a satisfactory level of confidence. This makes CIPC the epiphenomenal outcome of a cognitive process that is instrumental to the decision. Critically, our hypothesis implies novel predictions about how observed CIPC should relate to two specific meta-cognitive processes, namely: choice confidence and subjective certainty regarding pre-choice value judgments. We test these predictions in a behavioral experiment where participants rate the subjective value of food items both before and after choosing between equally valued items; we augment this traditional design with both reports of choice confidence and subjective certainty about value judgments. The results confirm our predictions and provide evidence that many quantitative features of CIPC (in particular: its relationship with metacognitive judgments) may be explained without ever invoking post-choice cognitive dissonance reduction explanation. We then discuss the relevance of our work in the context of the existing debate regarding the putative cognitive mechanisms underlying CIPC.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>32421699</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0231081</doi><tpages>e0231081</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5892-8694</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2020-05, Vol.15 (5), p.e0231081-e0231081 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_2404324146 |
source | Open Access: PubMed Central; Publicly Available Content Database |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Animal cognition Biology and Life Sciences Choice (Psychology) Choice Behavior - physiology Cognition Cognitive ability Cognitive Dissonance Confidence Context Decision making Decision Making - physiology Decision theory Emotions - physiology Female Food Food Preferences - psychology Humans Influence Judgments Life Sciences Male Medical imaging Metacognition Methods Middle Aged Neurons and Cognition Novels Personal preferences (Social sciences) Physical Sciences Predictions Preferences Prejudice Psychology and behavior Ratings & rankings Research and Analysis Methods Social Sciences Values Young Adult |
title | Choosing what we like vs liking what we choose: How choice-induced preference change might actually be instrumental to decision-making |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-30T19%3A10%3A15IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Choosing%20what%20we%20like%20vs%20liking%20what%20we%20choose:%20How%20choice-induced%20preference%20change%20might%20actually%20be%20instrumental%20to%20decision-making&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Lee,%20Douglas&rft.date=2020-05-18&rft.volume=15&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=e0231081&rft.epage=e0231081&rft.pages=e0231081-e0231081&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0231081&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA624289131%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c726t-be8a8ca834126af9187bca7073e87c75c594d4cb83556dcf227cfd694631c1433%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2404324146&rft_id=info:pmid/32421699&rft_galeid=A624289131&rfr_iscdi=true |