Loading…
The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials
The quality of evidence about the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical health interventions is often low, but little is known about the effects of communicating indications of evidence quality to the public. In two blinded, randomised, controlled, online experiments, US participants (total n = 2140)...
Saved in:
Published in: | PloS one 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0259048 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653 |
container_end_page | |
container_issue | 11 |
container_start_page | e0259048 |
container_title | PloS one |
container_volume | 16 |
creator | Schneider, Claudia R Freeman, Alexandra L J Spiegelhalter, David van der Linden, Sander |
description | The quality of evidence about the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical health interventions is often low, but little is known about the effects of communicating indications of evidence quality to the public.
In two blinded, randomised, controlled, online experiments, US participants (total n = 2140) were shown one of several versions of an infographic illustrating the effectiveness of eye protection in reducing COVID-19 transmission. Their trust in the information, understanding, feelings of effectiveness of eye protection, and the likelihood of them adopting it were measured.
Compared to those given no quality cues, participants who were told the quality of the evidence on eye protection was 'low', rated the evidence less trustworthy (p = .001, d = 0.25), and rated it as subjectively less effective (p = .018, d = 0.19). The same effects emerged compared to those who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high', and in one of the two studies, those shown 'low' quality of evidence said they were less likely to use eye protection (p = .005, d = 0.18). Participants who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high' showed no statistically significant differences on these measures compared to those given no information about evidence quality.
Without quality of evidence cues, participants responded to the evidence about the public health intervention as if it was high quality and this affected their subjective perceptions of its efficacy and trust in the provided information. This raises the ethical dilemma of weighing the importance of transparently stating when the evidence base is actually low quality against evidence that providing such information can decrease trust, perception of intervention efficacy, and likelihood of adopting it. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1371/journal.pone.0259048 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2598510767</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A682876751</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_0efcf9dd577f4a31b4b7ae4529f35239</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A682876751</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNptUl1rFDEUHUSxtfoPRAcE8WXXZDKZZHwQyvq1UOjL6mvIx81ulsxkmsxU-h_80WY729KKELg3ybkn99yconiN0RIThj_uwxR76ZdD6GGJKtqimj8pTnFLqkVTIfL0QX5SvEhpjxAlvGmeFyekZpxXbXta_NnsoARrQY-pDLa8mqR3480hhWtnoNdQ6tB1U--0HF3oy7wGiBqGeWfLYVLe6XIH0o-70vU2xG6GShWmsVxd_lp_WeD2U7n5HcooexM6l8Bk3n6MwfucjtFJn14Wz2wO8OoYz4qf375uVj8WF5ff16vzi4WmlI0Lq2ujKOVthbiy2LCshhCLGzCgGK6M5lqRBizVspGsplbhhtQKG6WBNZScFW9n3sGHJI6DTCLPkFOMWMMyYj0jTJB7MUTXyXgjgnTi9iDErZBxdNqDQGC1bY2hjNlaEqxqxSTUtGotoRVpM9fn42uT6sBoyKqlf0T6-KZ3O7EN14LnfhDhmeDDkSCGqwnSKPL8NHgvewjTbd8tYhXmOEPf_QP9v7r3M2ors4D541Lw0-HTkjhveMUzjB7o6hmoY0gpgr3vGiNxcOEdvTi4UBxdmMvePFR8X3RnO_IXov7diQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2598510767</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials</title><source>Publicly Available Content (ProQuest)</source><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Schneider, Claudia R ; Freeman, Alexandra L J ; Spiegelhalter, David ; van der Linden, Sander</creator><creatorcontrib>Schneider, Claudia R ; Freeman, Alexandra L J ; Spiegelhalter, David ; van der Linden, Sander</creatorcontrib><description>The quality of evidence about the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical health interventions is often low, but little is known about the effects of communicating indications of evidence quality to the public.
In two blinded, randomised, controlled, online experiments, US participants (total n = 2140) were shown one of several versions of an infographic illustrating the effectiveness of eye protection in reducing COVID-19 transmission. Their trust in the information, understanding, feelings of effectiveness of eye protection, and the likelihood of them adopting it were measured.
Compared to those given no quality cues, participants who were told the quality of the evidence on eye protection was 'low', rated the evidence less trustworthy (p = .001, d = 0.25), and rated it as subjectively less effective (p = .018, d = 0.19). The same effects emerged compared to those who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high', and in one of the two studies, those shown 'low' quality of evidence said they were less likely to use eye protection (p = .005, d = 0.18). Participants who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high' showed no statistically significant differences on these measures compared to those given no information about evidence quality.
Without quality of evidence cues, participants responded to the evidence about the public health intervention as if it was high quality and this affected their subjective perceptions of its efficacy and trust in the provided information. This raises the ethical dilemma of weighing the importance of transparently stating when the evidence base is actually low quality against evidence that providing such information can decrease trust, perception of intervention efficacy, and likelihood of adopting it.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259048</identifier><identifier>PMID: 34788299</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Aged ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Communication ; Computer and Information Sciences ; Confidence intervals ; Control ; Coronaviruses ; COVID-19 ; COVID-19 - epidemiology ; COVID-19 - prevention & control ; COVID-19 - psychology ; Disease transmission ; Effectiveness ; Epidemics ; Eye protection ; Female ; Health promotion ; Humans ; Infections ; Information management ; Intervention ; Male ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Middle Aged ; Pandemics ; Perception ; Personal health ; Pharmaceuticals ; Psychology ; Public Health ; SARS-CoV-2 ; Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 ; Social Sciences ; Statistical analysis ; Trust ; Trustworthiness ; United Kingdom ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0259048</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2021 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2021 Schneider et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2021 Schneider et al 2021 Schneider et al</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-4115-161X ; 0000-0002-6612-5186</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2598510767/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2598510767?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,881,25731,27901,27902,36989,36990,44566,53766,53768,75096</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34788299$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Schneider, Claudia R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Freeman, Alexandra L J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spiegelhalter, David</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van der Linden, Sander</creatorcontrib><title>The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>The quality of evidence about the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical health interventions is often low, but little is known about the effects of communicating indications of evidence quality to the public.
In two blinded, randomised, controlled, online experiments, US participants (total n = 2140) were shown one of several versions of an infographic illustrating the effectiveness of eye protection in reducing COVID-19 transmission. Their trust in the information, understanding, feelings of effectiveness of eye protection, and the likelihood of them adopting it were measured.
Compared to those given no quality cues, participants who were told the quality of the evidence on eye protection was 'low', rated the evidence less trustworthy (p = .001, d = 0.25), and rated it as subjectively less effective (p = .018, d = 0.19). The same effects emerged compared to those who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high', and in one of the two studies, those shown 'low' quality of evidence said they were less likely to use eye protection (p = .005, d = 0.18). Participants who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high' showed no statistically significant differences on these measures compared to those given no information about evidence quality.
Without quality of evidence cues, participants responded to the evidence about the public health intervention as if it was high quality and this affected their subjective perceptions of its efficacy and trust in the provided information. This raises the ethical dilemma of weighing the importance of transparently stating when the evidence base is actually low quality against evidence that providing such information can decrease trust, perception of intervention efficacy, and likelihood of adopting it.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Communication</subject><subject>Computer and Information Sciences</subject><subject>Confidence intervals</subject><subject>Control</subject><subject>Coronaviruses</subject><subject>COVID-19</subject><subject>COVID-19 - epidemiology</subject><subject>COVID-19 - prevention & control</subject><subject>COVID-19 - psychology</subject><subject>Disease transmission</subject><subject>Effectiveness</subject><subject>Epidemics</subject><subject>Eye protection</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Health promotion</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infections</subject><subject>Information management</subject><subject>Intervention</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Pandemics</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Personal health</subject><subject>Pharmaceuticals</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Public Health</subject><subject>SARS-CoV-2</subject><subject>Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Statistical analysis</subject><subject>Trust</subject><subject>Trustworthiness</subject><subject>United Kingdom</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2021</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNptUl1rFDEUHUSxtfoPRAcE8WXXZDKZZHwQyvq1UOjL6mvIx81ulsxkmsxU-h_80WY729KKELg3ybkn99yconiN0RIThj_uwxR76ZdD6GGJKtqimj8pTnFLqkVTIfL0QX5SvEhpjxAlvGmeFyekZpxXbXta_NnsoARrQY-pDLa8mqR3480hhWtnoNdQ6tB1U--0HF3oy7wGiBqGeWfLYVLe6XIH0o-70vU2xG6GShWmsVxd_lp_WeD2U7n5HcooexM6l8Bk3n6MwfucjtFJn14Wz2wO8OoYz4qf375uVj8WF5ff16vzi4WmlI0Lq2ujKOVthbiy2LCshhCLGzCgGK6M5lqRBizVspGsplbhhtQKG6WBNZScFW9n3sGHJI6DTCLPkFOMWMMyYj0jTJB7MUTXyXgjgnTi9iDErZBxdNqDQGC1bY2hjNlaEqxqxSTUtGotoRVpM9fn42uT6sBoyKqlf0T6-KZ3O7EN14LnfhDhmeDDkSCGqwnSKPL8NHgvewjTbd8tYhXmOEPf_QP9v7r3M2ors4D541Lw0-HTkjhveMUzjB7o6hmoY0gpgr3vGiNxcOEdvTi4UBxdmMvePFR8X3RnO_IXov7diQ</recordid><startdate>20211117</startdate><enddate>20211117</enddate><creator>Schneider, Claudia R</creator><creator>Freeman, Alexandra L J</creator><creator>Spiegelhalter, David</creator><creator>van der Linden, Sander</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PHGZM</scope><scope>PHGZT</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PJZUB</scope><scope>PKEHL</scope><scope>PPXIY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQGLB</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4115-161X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6612-5186</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20211117</creationdate><title>The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials</title><author>Schneider, Claudia R ; Freeman, Alexandra L J ; Spiegelhalter, David ; van der Linden, Sander</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2021</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Communication</topic><topic>Computer and Information Sciences</topic><topic>Confidence intervals</topic><topic>Control</topic><topic>Coronaviruses</topic><topic>COVID-19</topic><topic>COVID-19 - epidemiology</topic><topic>COVID-19 - prevention & control</topic><topic>COVID-19 - psychology</topic><topic>Disease transmission</topic><topic>Effectiveness</topic><topic>Epidemics</topic><topic>Eye protection</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Health promotion</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infections</topic><topic>Information management</topic><topic>Intervention</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Pandemics</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Personal health</topic><topic>Pharmaceuticals</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Public Health</topic><topic>SARS-CoV-2</topic><topic>Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Statistical analysis</topic><topic>Trust</topic><topic>Trustworthiness</topic><topic>United Kingdom</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Schneider, Claudia R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Freeman, Alexandra L J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spiegelhalter, David</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van der Linden, Sander</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database (Proquest)</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science & Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database (1962 - current)</collection><collection>ProQuest Agriculture & Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Biological Sciences</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>PML(ProQuest Medical Library)</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>ProQuest advanced technologies & aerospace journals</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic (New)</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Health & Nursing</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Engineering collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Schneider, Claudia R</au><au>Freeman, Alexandra L J</au><au>Spiegelhalter, David</au><au>van der Linden, Sander</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2021-11-17</date><risdate>2021</risdate><volume>16</volume><issue>11</issue><spage>e0259048</spage><pages>e0259048-</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>The quality of evidence about the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical health interventions is often low, but little is known about the effects of communicating indications of evidence quality to the public.
In two blinded, randomised, controlled, online experiments, US participants (total n = 2140) were shown one of several versions of an infographic illustrating the effectiveness of eye protection in reducing COVID-19 transmission. Their trust in the information, understanding, feelings of effectiveness of eye protection, and the likelihood of them adopting it were measured.
Compared to those given no quality cues, participants who were told the quality of the evidence on eye protection was 'low', rated the evidence less trustworthy (p = .001, d = 0.25), and rated it as subjectively less effective (p = .018, d = 0.19). The same effects emerged compared to those who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high', and in one of the two studies, those shown 'low' quality of evidence said they were less likely to use eye protection (p = .005, d = 0.18). Participants who were told the quality of the evidence was 'high' showed no statistically significant differences on these measures compared to those given no information about evidence quality.
Without quality of evidence cues, participants responded to the evidence about the public health intervention as if it was high quality and this affected their subjective perceptions of its efficacy and trust in the provided information. This raises the ethical dilemma of weighing the importance of transparently stating when the evidence base is actually low quality against evidence that providing such information can decrease trust, perception of intervention efficacy, and likelihood of adopting it.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>34788299</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0259048</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4115-161X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6612-5186</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1932-6203 |
ispartof | PloS one, 2021-11, Vol.16 (11), p.e0259048 |
issn | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_plos_journals_2598510767 |
source | Publicly Available Content (ProQuest); PubMed Central |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Aged Biology and Life Sciences Communication Computer and Information Sciences Confidence intervals Control Coronaviruses COVID-19 COVID-19 - epidemiology COVID-19 - prevention & control COVID-19 - psychology Disease transmission Effectiveness Epidemics Eye protection Female Health promotion Humans Infections Information management Intervention Male Medicine and Health Sciences Middle Aged Pandemics Perception Personal health Pharmaceuticals Psychology Public Health SARS-CoV-2 Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 Social Sciences Statistical analysis Trust Trustworthiness United Kingdom Young Adult |
title | The effects of quality of evidence communication on perception of public health information about COVID-19: Two randomised controlled trials |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-23T19%3A48%3A54IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20effects%20of%20quality%20of%20evidence%20communication%20on%20perception%20of%20public%20health%20information%20about%20COVID-19:%20Two%20randomised%20controlled%20trials&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=Schneider,%20Claudia%20R&rft.date=2021-11-17&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=e0259048&rft.pages=e0259048-&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0259048&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA682876751%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c557t-fc4db5589208bf1d747833f16edeb712dc8cb36ef5ca6a745fb1634b1dbce7653%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2598510767&rft_id=info:pmid/34788299&rft_galeid=A682876751&rfr_iscdi=true |