Loading…

The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood

The processes involved in the Stroop task/effect are thought to involve conflict detection and resolution stages. Little is known about the evolution of these two components over the lifespan. It is well admitted that children and older adults tend to show longer response latencies than young adults...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:PloS one 2023-03, Vol.18 (3), p.e0256003-e0256003
Main Authors: Ménétré, Eric, Laganaro, Marina
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3
container_end_page e0256003
container_issue 3
container_start_page e0256003
container_title PloS one
container_volume 18
creator Ménétré, Eric
Laganaro, Marina
description The processes involved in the Stroop task/effect are thought to involve conflict detection and resolution stages. Little is known about the evolution of these two components over the lifespan. It is well admitted that children and older adults tend to show longer response latencies than young adults. The present study aims at clarifying the rational of such changes from childhood to adulthood and in aging by comparing the impacted cognitive processes across age groups. More precisely, the aim was to clarify if all processes take more time to be executed, hence implying that longer latencies rely mainly on processing speed or if an additional process lengthens the resolution of the conflict in children and/or older adults. To this aim we recorded brain electrical activity using EEG in school-age children, young and older adults while they performed a classic verbal Stroop task. The signal was decomposed in microstate brain networks, and age groups and conditions were compared. Behavioral results evolved following an inverted U-shaped curve. In children, different brain states from the ones observed in adults were highlighted, both in the conflict detection and resolution time-windows. Longer latencies in the incongruent condition were mainly attributed to an overly increased duration of the microstates involved in the conflict resolution time window. In aging, the same microstate maps were reported for both young and older adult groups. The differences in performances between groups could be explained by a disproportionally long conflict detection phase, even compressing the latest stage of response articulation. These results tend to favor a specific immaturity of the brain networks involved coupled with a slowing of the processes in children, while cognitive decline could be mostly explained by a general slowing.
doi_str_mv 10.1371/journal.pone.0256003
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_plos_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_plos_journals_2792882756</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A743645990</galeid><doaj_id>oai_doaj_org_article_1ebcf4593fa541d58fc6187e13d3866f</doaj_id><sourcerecordid>A743645990</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkl1r2zAUhs3YWNts_2BsgkHZLpLpw1asq1HKPgKFwtr1VsjSUeJgW6kkj-XfT17cEo9eDF_I6DznPdKrN8veELwgbEk-bV3vO9Usdq6DBaYFx5g9y06JYHTOKWbPj_5PsrMQthgXrOT8ZXbCuBAc5-Vpdne7ARSh3TmvGmT2nWprHZCzKKbCTfTO7RBYCzoi612L9KZuzMY5g6JDe9d3a6Q6g1xjwCNl-iYOxVfZC6uaAK_HdZb9_Prl9vL7_Or62-ry4mquuWBxzgtNDSia48IarKvSloJUKs9NSbC1Ip1f0bLImaAUVGlEXlBBcmBVpakFYLPs3UF317ggR0uCpEtBy5IuC56I1YEwTm3lztet8nvpVC3_bji_lsrHWjcgCVTa5oVgVhU5MUVpNSflEggzg282aX0ep_VVC0ZDF5NrE9Fppas3cu1-SYIxp7zASeHDqODdfQ8hyrYOGppGdeD6w8EF5ji5M8ve_4M-fb2RWqt0g7qzLg3Wg6i8WOaMp-uIYeziCSp9BtJzpwDZOu1PGj5OGhIT4Xdcqz4Eubr58f_s9d2UPT9iN6BSXIJr-li7LkzB_ABq70LwYB9dJlgO-X9wQw75l2P-U9vb4xd6bHoIPPsDiXf_uw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Website</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2792882756</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood</title><source>PubMed Central Free</source><source>Publicly Available Content Database</source><creator>Ménétré, Eric ; Laganaro, Marina</creator><contributor>Fernandez, Thalia</contributor><creatorcontrib>Ménétré, Eric ; Laganaro, Marina ; Fernandez, Thalia</creatorcontrib><description>The processes involved in the Stroop task/effect are thought to involve conflict detection and resolution stages. Little is known about the evolution of these two components over the lifespan. It is well admitted that children and older adults tend to show longer response latencies than young adults. The present study aims at clarifying the rational of such changes from childhood to adulthood and in aging by comparing the impacted cognitive processes across age groups. More precisely, the aim was to clarify if all processes take more time to be executed, hence implying that longer latencies rely mainly on processing speed or if an additional process lengthens the resolution of the conflict in children and/or older adults. To this aim we recorded brain electrical activity using EEG in school-age children, young and older adults while they performed a classic verbal Stroop task. The signal was decomposed in microstate brain networks, and age groups and conditions were compared. Behavioral results evolved following an inverted U-shaped curve. In children, different brain states from the ones observed in adults were highlighted, both in the conflict detection and resolution time-windows. Longer latencies in the incongruent condition were mainly attributed to an overly increased duration of the microstates involved in the conflict resolution time window. In aging, the same microstate maps were reported for both young and older adult groups. The differences in performances between groups could be explained by a disproportionally long conflict detection phase, even compressing the latest stage of response articulation. These results tend to favor a specific immaturity of the brain networks involved coupled with a slowing of the processes in children, while cognitive decline could be mostly explained by a general slowing.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1932-6203</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256003</identifier><identifier>PMID: 36996048</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Public Library of Science</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adults ; Age ; Aged ; Aging ; Aging - physiology ; Analysis ; Biology and Life Sciences ; Brain ; Brain - physiology ; Brain Mapping ; Child ; Childhood ; Children ; Cognition ; Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Cognitive ability ; Computer and Information Sciences ; Conflict management ; Conflict resolution ; Demographic aspects ; EEG ; Electroencephalography ; Evaluation ; Evolution ; Humans ; Hypotheses ; Life span ; Medical imaging ; Medicine and Health Sciences ; Neuroimaging ; Older people ; People and Places ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Reading ; Research and Analysis Methods ; Stroop effect ; Stroop Test ; Windows (intervals) ; Young Adult ; Young adults</subject><ispartof>PloS one, 2023-03, Vol.18 (3), p.e0256003-e0256003</ispartof><rights>Copyright: © 2023 Ménétré, Laganaro. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2023 Public Library of Science</rights><rights>2023 Ménétré, Laganaro. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2023 Ménétré, Laganaro 2023 Ménétré, Laganaro</rights><rights>2023 Ménétré, Laganaro. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-1101-1288</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2792882756/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2792882756?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,25753,27924,27925,37012,37013,44590,53791,53793,75126</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36996048$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Fernandez, Thalia</contributor><creatorcontrib>Ménétré, Eric</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Laganaro, Marina</creatorcontrib><title>The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood</title><title>PloS one</title><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><description>The processes involved in the Stroop task/effect are thought to involve conflict detection and resolution stages. Little is known about the evolution of these two components over the lifespan. It is well admitted that children and older adults tend to show longer response latencies than young adults. The present study aims at clarifying the rational of such changes from childhood to adulthood and in aging by comparing the impacted cognitive processes across age groups. More precisely, the aim was to clarify if all processes take more time to be executed, hence implying that longer latencies rely mainly on processing speed or if an additional process lengthens the resolution of the conflict in children and/or older adults. To this aim we recorded brain electrical activity using EEG in school-age children, young and older adults while they performed a classic verbal Stroop task. The signal was decomposed in microstate brain networks, and age groups and conditions were compared. Behavioral results evolved following an inverted U-shaped curve. In children, different brain states from the ones observed in adults were highlighted, both in the conflict detection and resolution time-windows. Longer latencies in the incongruent condition were mainly attributed to an overly increased duration of the microstates involved in the conflict resolution time window. In aging, the same microstate maps were reported for both young and older adult groups. The differences in performances between groups could be explained by a disproportionally long conflict detection phase, even compressing the latest stage of response articulation. These results tend to favor a specific immaturity of the brain networks involved coupled with a slowing of the processes in children, while cognitive decline could be mostly explained by a general slowing.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Age</subject><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aging</subject><subject>Aging - physiology</subject><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Brain Mapping</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Childhood</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Computer and Information Sciences</subject><subject>Conflict management</subject><subject>Conflict resolution</subject><subject>Demographic aspects</subject><subject>EEG</subject><subject>Electroencephalography</subject><subject>Evaluation</subject><subject>Evolution</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hypotheses</subject><subject>Life span</subject><subject>Medical imaging</subject><subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject><subject>Neuroimaging</subject><subject>Older people</subject><subject>People and Places</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Research and Analysis Methods</subject><subject>Stroop effect</subject><subject>Stroop Test</subject><subject>Windows (intervals)</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><subject>Young adults</subject><issn>1932-6203</issn><issn>1932-6203</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>PIMPY</sourceid><sourceid>DOA</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkl1r2zAUhs3YWNts_2BsgkHZLpLpw1asq1HKPgKFwtr1VsjSUeJgW6kkj-XfT17cEo9eDF_I6DznPdKrN8veELwgbEk-bV3vO9Usdq6DBaYFx5g9y06JYHTOKWbPj_5PsrMQthgXrOT8ZXbCuBAc5-Vpdne7ARSh3TmvGmT2nWprHZCzKKbCTfTO7RBYCzoi612L9KZuzMY5g6JDe9d3a6Q6g1xjwCNl-iYOxVfZC6uaAK_HdZb9_Prl9vL7_Or62-ry4mquuWBxzgtNDSia48IarKvSloJUKs9NSbC1Ip1f0bLImaAUVGlEXlBBcmBVpakFYLPs3UF317ggR0uCpEtBy5IuC56I1YEwTm3lztet8nvpVC3_bji_lsrHWjcgCVTa5oVgVhU5MUVpNSflEggzg282aX0ep_VVC0ZDF5NrE9Fppas3cu1-SYIxp7zASeHDqODdfQ8hyrYOGppGdeD6w8EF5ji5M8ve_4M-fb2RWqt0g7qzLg3Wg6i8WOaMp-uIYeziCSp9BtJzpwDZOu1PGj5OGhIT4Xdcqz4Eubr58f_s9d2UPT9iN6BSXIJr-li7LkzB_ABq70LwYB9dJlgO-X9wQw75l2P-U9vb4xd6bHoIPPsDiXf_uw</recordid><startdate>20230330</startdate><enddate>20230330</enddate><creator>Ménétré, Eric</creator><creator>Laganaro, Marina</creator><general>Public Library of Science</general><general>Public Library of Science (PLoS)</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOV</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TG</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X2</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8C1</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FG</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABJCF</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ARAPS</scope><scope>ATCPS</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGLVJ</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>D1I</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>KL.</scope><scope>L6V</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0K</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>M7S</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P5Z</scope><scope>P62</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PATMY</scope><scope>PDBOC</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PTHSS</scope><scope>PYCSY</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>DOA</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1101-1288</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20230330</creationdate><title>The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood</title><author>Ménétré, Eric ; Laganaro, Marina</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Age</topic><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aging</topic><topic>Aging - physiology</topic><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Biology and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Brain Mapping</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Childhood</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Computer and Information Sciences</topic><topic>Conflict management</topic><topic>Conflict resolution</topic><topic>Demographic aspects</topic><topic>EEG</topic><topic>Electroencephalography</topic><topic>Evaluation</topic><topic>Evolution</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hypotheses</topic><topic>Life span</topic><topic>Medical imaging</topic><topic>Medicine and Health Sciences</topic><topic>Neuroimaging</topic><topic>Older people</topic><topic>People and Places</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Research and Analysis Methods</topic><topic>Stroop effect</topic><topic>Stroop Test</topic><topic>Windows (intervals)</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><topic>Young adults</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ménétré, Eric</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Laganaro, Marina</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Agricultural Science Collection</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Public Health Database</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Technology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Materials Science &amp; Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Agricultural &amp; Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Technology Collection</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Materials Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Meteorological &amp; Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest Engineering Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Agriculture Science Database</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Engineering Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Advanced Technologies &amp; Aerospace Collection</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Environmental Science Database</collection><collection>Materials Science Collection</collection><collection>Publicly Available Content Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Engineering Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Science Collection</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals</collection><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ménétré, Eric</au><au>Laganaro, Marina</au><au>Fernandez, Thalia</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood</atitle><jtitle>PloS one</jtitle><addtitle>PLoS One</addtitle><date>2023-03-30</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>18</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>e0256003</spage><epage>e0256003</epage><pages>e0256003-e0256003</pages><issn>1932-6203</issn><eissn>1932-6203</eissn><abstract>The processes involved in the Stroop task/effect are thought to involve conflict detection and resolution stages. Little is known about the evolution of these two components over the lifespan. It is well admitted that children and older adults tend to show longer response latencies than young adults. The present study aims at clarifying the rational of such changes from childhood to adulthood and in aging by comparing the impacted cognitive processes across age groups. More precisely, the aim was to clarify if all processes take more time to be executed, hence implying that longer latencies rely mainly on processing speed or if an additional process lengthens the resolution of the conflict in children and/or older adults. To this aim we recorded brain electrical activity using EEG in school-age children, young and older adults while they performed a classic verbal Stroop task. The signal was decomposed in microstate brain networks, and age groups and conditions were compared. Behavioral results evolved following an inverted U-shaped curve. In children, different brain states from the ones observed in adults were highlighted, both in the conflict detection and resolution time-windows. Longer latencies in the incongruent condition were mainly attributed to an overly increased duration of the microstates involved in the conflict resolution time window. In aging, the same microstate maps were reported for both young and older adult groups. The differences in performances between groups could be explained by a disproportionally long conflict detection phase, even compressing the latest stage of response articulation. These results tend to favor a specific immaturity of the brain networks involved coupled with a slowing of the processes in children, while cognitive decline could be mostly explained by a general slowing.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Public Library of Science</pub><pmid>36996048</pmid><doi>10.1371/journal.pone.0256003</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1101-1288</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1932-6203
ispartof PloS one, 2023-03, Vol.18 (3), p.e0256003-e0256003
issn 1932-6203
1932-6203
language eng
recordid cdi_plos_journals_2792882756
source PubMed Central Free; Publicly Available Content Database
subjects Adolescent
Adults
Age
Aged
Aging
Aging - physiology
Analysis
Biology and Life Sciences
Brain
Brain - physiology
Brain Mapping
Child
Childhood
Children
Cognition
Cognition & reasoning
Cognitive ability
Computer and Information Sciences
Conflict management
Conflict resolution
Demographic aspects
EEG
Electroencephalography
Evaluation
Evolution
Humans
Hypotheses
Life span
Medical imaging
Medicine and Health Sciences
Neuroimaging
Older people
People and Places
Reaction Time - physiology
Reading
Research and Analysis Methods
Stroop effect
Stroop Test
Windows (intervals)
Young Adult
Young adults
title The temporal dynamics of the Stroop effect from childhood to young and older adulthood
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-04T03%3A21%3A12IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_plos_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20temporal%20dynamics%20of%20the%20Stroop%20effect%20from%20childhood%20to%20young%20and%20older%20adulthood&rft.jtitle=PloS%20one&rft.au=M%C3%A9n%C3%A9tr%C3%A9,%20Eric&rft.date=2023-03-30&rft.volume=18&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=e0256003&rft.epage=e0256003&rft.pages=e0256003-e0256003&rft.issn=1932-6203&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0256003&rft_dat=%3Cgale_plos_%3EA743645990%3C/gale_plos_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c693t-65c2dea2405fd0cb8f891ba44d810ff9932a28543922ea8d9452914e3bbc2fee3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2792882756&rft_id=info:pmid/36996048&rft_galeid=A743645990&rfr_iscdi=true