Loading…

Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances

A series of experiments was carried out to examine memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of sentences containing reported utterances. The illocutionary force of an utterance corresponds roughly to the intent of the speaker (to warn, to ask, to order). The perlocutionary effect i...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 1976-06, Vol.15 (3), p.325-337
Main Authors: Schweller, Kenneth G., Brewer, William F., Dahl, Deborah A.
Format: Article
Language:English
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963
container_end_page 337
container_issue 3
container_start_page 325
container_title Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
container_volume 15
creator Schweller, Kenneth G.
Brewer, William F.
Dahl, Deborah A.
description A series of experiments was carried out to examine memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of sentences containing reported utterances. The illocutionary force of an utterance corresponds roughly to the intent of the speaker (to warn, to ask, to order). The perlocutionary effect is the effect the utterance might have on a hearer (to amaze, to bore, to frighten). Since understanding the illocutionary forces and the perlocutionary effects of utterances appears to be a central part of using language, it was hypothesized that subjects hearing sentences containing reported utterances would confuse these original sentences with new sentences containing illocutionary forces or perlocutionary effects consistent with the original sentences. The predicted effects were found in recall for illocutionary forces and in recognition memory for perlocutionary effects.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90029-3
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1297344459</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>0022537176900293</els_id><sourcerecordid>1297344459</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9UE1LAzEQzUHBWv0HHha86GE1k8_NRZDiF1S86Dmk2VlIaTc12RX896auiCdPw8y892beI-QM6BVQUNeUMlZLruFCq0tTOlPzAzL7HR-R45zXlHKARs7I4zNuY_qsupiqsNlEPw4h9m6aeMyV69tqh-nvBrsO_ZCr2FXjMGByfQGekMPObTKe_tQ5ebu_e1081suXh6fF7bL2TOih9ivWrLhT0gE62WgNIL0Dz5QwzivWtFoqx8oBBmaFShvFJQjGQUqtSjMn55PuLsX3EfNg13FMfTlpgRnNhRDSFJSYUD7FnBN2dpfCtjxvgdp9TnYfiN0HYrWy3zlZXmg3Ew2Lg4-AyWYfsNhrQyqWbRvD_wJfXWZwZA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1297344459</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances</title><source>Periodicals Archive Online JISC Collection</source><source>ScienceDirect: Social Science Backfile</source><source>Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection</source><creator>Schweller, Kenneth G. ; Brewer, William F. ; Dahl, Deborah A.</creator><creatorcontrib>Schweller, Kenneth G. ; Brewer, William F. ; Dahl, Deborah A.</creatorcontrib><description>A series of experiments was carried out to examine memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of sentences containing reported utterances. The illocutionary force of an utterance corresponds roughly to the intent of the speaker (to warn, to ask, to order). The perlocutionary effect is the effect the utterance might have on a hearer (to amaze, to bore, to frighten). Since understanding the illocutionary forces and the perlocutionary effects of utterances appears to be a central part of using language, it was hypothesized that subjects hearing sentences containing reported utterances would confuse these original sentences with new sentences containing illocutionary forces or perlocutionary effects consistent with the original sentences. The predicted effects were found in recall for illocutionary forces and in recognition memory for perlocutionary effects.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-5371</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 0749-596X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90029-3</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Elsevier B.V</publisher><ispartof>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1976-06, Vol.15 (3), p.325-337</ispartof><rights>1976 Academic Press, Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1297344459/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1297344459?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3642,13385,13412,27903,27904,38593,38728,44733,44736,46009,75044,75047</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Schweller, Kenneth G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brewer, William F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dahl, Deborah A.</creatorcontrib><title>Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances</title><title>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior</title><description>A series of experiments was carried out to examine memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of sentences containing reported utterances. The illocutionary force of an utterance corresponds roughly to the intent of the speaker (to warn, to ask, to order). The perlocutionary effect is the effect the utterance might have on a hearer (to amaze, to bore, to frighten). Since understanding the illocutionary forces and the perlocutionary effects of utterances appears to be a central part of using language, it was hypothesized that subjects hearing sentences containing reported utterances would confuse these original sentences with new sentences containing illocutionary forces or perlocutionary effects consistent with the original sentences. The predicted effects were found in recall for illocutionary forces and in recognition memory for perlocutionary effects.</description><issn>0022-5371</issn><issn>0749-596X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1976</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>~PJ</sourceid><sourceid>~PM</sourceid><recordid>eNp9UE1LAzEQzUHBWv0HHha86GE1k8_NRZDiF1S86Dmk2VlIaTc12RX896auiCdPw8y892beI-QM6BVQUNeUMlZLruFCq0tTOlPzAzL7HR-R45zXlHKARs7I4zNuY_qsupiqsNlEPw4h9m6aeMyV69tqh-nvBrsO_ZCr2FXjMGByfQGekMPObTKe_tQ5ebu_e1081suXh6fF7bL2TOih9ivWrLhT0gE62WgNIL0Dz5QwzivWtFoqx8oBBmaFShvFJQjGQUqtSjMn55PuLsX3EfNg13FMfTlpgRnNhRDSFJSYUD7FnBN2dpfCtjxvgdp9TnYfiN0HYrWy3zlZXmg3Ew2Lg4-AyWYfsNhrQyqWbRvD_wJfXWZwZA</recordid><startdate>19760601</startdate><enddate>19760601</enddate><creator>Schweller, Kenneth G.</creator><creator>Brewer, William F.</creator><creator>Dahl, Deborah A.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Academic Press</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0R3</scope><scope>ACFII</scope><scope>ANHVI</scope><scope>FUVTR</scope><scope>HYQOX</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>JSICY</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SFNNT</scope><scope>~OB</scope><scope>~OC</scope><scope>~OG</scope><scope>~PJ</scope><scope>~PM</scope><scope>~PN</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19760601</creationdate><title>Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances</title><author>Schweller, Kenneth G. ; Brewer, William F. ; Dahl, Deborah A.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1976</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Schweller, Kenneth G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brewer, William F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dahl, Deborah A.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Collection 1</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection 1 (2022)</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Liberal Arts Collection 1 (2022)</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 06</collection><collection>ProQuest Historical Periodicals</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 36</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 44</collection><collection>PAO Collection 1</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Collection 1</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online (1770-1995) [full page reproduction]</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online JISC Collection</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Liberal Arts Collection 1</collection><jtitle>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Schweller, Kenneth G.</au><au>Brewer, William F.</au><au>Dahl, Deborah A.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances</atitle><jtitle>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior</jtitle><date>1976-06-01</date><risdate>1976</risdate><volume>15</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>325</spage><epage>337</epage><pages>325-337</pages><issn>0022-5371</issn><issn>0749-596X</issn><abstract>A series of experiments was carried out to examine memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of sentences containing reported utterances. The illocutionary force of an utterance corresponds roughly to the intent of the speaker (to warn, to ask, to order). The perlocutionary effect is the effect the utterance might have on a hearer (to amaze, to bore, to frighten). Since understanding the illocutionary forces and the perlocutionary effects of utterances appears to be a central part of using language, it was hypothesized that subjects hearing sentences containing reported utterances would confuse these original sentences with new sentences containing illocutionary forces or perlocutionary effects consistent with the original sentences. The predicted effects were found in recall for illocutionary forces and in recognition memory for perlocutionary effects.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/0022-5371(76)90029-3</doi><tpages>13</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0022-5371
ispartof Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1976-06, Vol.15 (3), p.325-337
issn 0022-5371
0749-596X
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1297344459
source Periodicals Archive Online JISC Collection; ScienceDirect: Social Science Backfile; Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection
title Memory for illocutionary forces and perlocutionary effects of utterances
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-24T04%3A36%3A38IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Memory%20for%20illocutionary%20forces%20and%20perlocutionary%20effects%20of%20utterances&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20Verbal%20Learning%20and%20Verbal%20Behavior&rft.au=Schweller,%20Kenneth%20G.&rft.date=1976-06-01&rft.volume=15&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=325&rft.epage=337&rft.pages=325-337&rft.issn=0022-5371&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/0022-5371(76)90029-3&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1297344459%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c247t-cb28b3a65a1ea5877115ca1c2649ac628d756a2fec219be679635142315576963%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1297344459&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true