Loading…

Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)

Reports an error in "A further look at the "language-as-fixed-effect fallacy"" by Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers ( Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 2003[Sep], Vol 57[3], 141-151). The corrected version of Table 7 is listed below...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Canadian journal of experimental psychology 2013-03, Vol.67 (1), p.59-59
Main Author: Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by
cites
container_end_page 59
container_issue 1
container_start_page 59
container_title Canadian journal of experimental psychology
container_volume 67
creator Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.
description Reports an error in "A further look at the "language-as-fixed-effect fallacy"" by Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers ( Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 2003[Sep], Vol 57[3], 141-151). The corrected version of Table 7 is listed below. The accompanying text on p. 147 should read: “For these data, F’ = 5.95 ( df= 1,7). Using either the statistics from Table 7 or by running separate subjects and item analyses, we obtain F 1= 1157.1 ( df= 1,3) and F₂= 5.97 ( df= 1,7). This leads to minF ’ 5.94 ( df =1,7).” The conclusions drawn from this example are not affected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2003-08374-003.) The proper analysis of experiments using language materials has been a source of controversy and debate among researchers. We summarize the main issues and discuss the solutions that have been presented. Even though the major issues have been dealt with extensively in the literature, there still exists quite a bit of confusion about how to analyze the data from such experiments. We discuss a number of the most frequently voiced objections. In particular, we discuss the issue of what happens if in a counterbalanced design only some of the items show the treatment effect. Finally, a possible solution is discussed for the case where only partial matching of items between conditions is possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved) (Source: journal abstract)
doi_str_mv 10.1037/a0032114
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1326320285</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1314700017</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a1525-8a37eccb17f13d0798660452e2fe589f1d0146f8c70f2b32093a0358a2bc64f53</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0E1LAzEQBuAgCtYq-BMqXqqwOpNsPvYoi19QEETPYTZNYGvbXZPtof_eSBXRi4chc3gyw7yMnSJcIQh9TQCCI5Z7bIRGm0LzCvZzj5UqcuEhO0ppAZBxiSN2Vncxeje03TpNhm7yTNQuVvTmY5pMeZ51ccwOAi2TP_l6x-z17valfihmT_eP9c2sIJRcFoaE9s41qAOKOejKKAWl5J4HL00VcA5YqmCchsAbwaESBEIa4o1TZZBizM53c_vYvW98Guyi28R1XmlRcJV_cPOPwlJDvkxnNd0pF7uUog-2j-2K4tYi2M-Y7HdMmV7uKPVk-7R1FIfWLX1ymxzMerDO91Zpi1ZWP9v_4l_qAyDzb_s</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1314700017</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)</title><source>PsycARTICLES</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection</source><creator>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</creator><contributor>Mewhort, Douglas J. K</contributor><creatorcontrib>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W. ; Mewhort, Douglas J. K</creatorcontrib><description>Reports an error in "A further look at the "language-as-fixed-effect fallacy"" by Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers ( Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 2003[Sep], Vol 57[3], 141-151). The corrected version of Table 7 is listed below. The accompanying text on p. 147 should read: “For these data, F’ = 5.95 ( df= 1,7). Using either the statistics from Table 7 or by running separate subjects and item analyses, we obtain F 1= 1157.1 ( df= 1,3) and F₂= 5.97 ( df= 1,7). This leads to minF ’ 5.94 ( df =1,7).” The conclusions drawn from this example are not affected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2003-08374-003.) The proper analysis of experiments using language materials has been a source of controversy and debate among researchers. We summarize the main issues and discuss the solutions that have been presented. Even though the major issues have been dealt with extensively in the literature, there still exists quite a bit of confusion about how to analyze the data from such experiments. We discuss a number of the most frequently voiced objections. In particular, we discuss the issue of what happens if in a counterbalanced design only some of the items show the treatment effect. Finally, a possible solution is discussed for the case where only partial matching of items between conditions is possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved) (Source: journal abstract)</description><identifier>ISSN: 1196-1961</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1878-7290</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/a0032114</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CJEPEK</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Ottawa: Educational Publishing Foundation</publisher><subject>Experimental Design ; Language ; Statistical Analysis</subject><ispartof>Canadian journal of experimental psychology, 2013-03, Vol.67 (1), p.59-59</ispartof><rights>2013 Canadian Psychological Association</rights><rights>2013, Canadian Psychological Association</rights><rights>Copyright Canadian Psychological Association Mar 2013</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1326320285?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21394,27924,27925,33611,43733</link.rule.ids></links><search><contributor>Mewhort, Douglas J. K</contributor><creatorcontrib>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</creatorcontrib><title>Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)</title><title>Canadian journal of experimental psychology</title><description>Reports an error in "A further look at the "language-as-fixed-effect fallacy"" by Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers ( Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 2003[Sep], Vol 57[3], 141-151). The corrected version of Table 7 is listed below. The accompanying text on p. 147 should read: “For these data, F’ = 5.95 ( df= 1,7). Using either the statistics from Table 7 or by running separate subjects and item analyses, we obtain F 1= 1157.1 ( df= 1,3) and F₂= 5.97 ( df= 1,7). This leads to minF ’ 5.94 ( df =1,7).” The conclusions drawn from this example are not affected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2003-08374-003.) The proper analysis of experiments using language materials has been a source of controversy and debate among researchers. We summarize the main issues and discuss the solutions that have been presented. Even though the major issues have been dealt with extensively in the literature, there still exists quite a bit of confusion about how to analyze the data from such experiments. We discuss a number of the most frequently voiced objections. In particular, we discuss the issue of what happens if in a counterbalanced design only some of the items show the treatment effect. Finally, a possible solution is discussed for the case where only partial matching of items between conditions is possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved) (Source: journal abstract)</description><subject>Experimental Design</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Statistical Analysis</subject><issn>1196-1961</issn><issn>1878-7290</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2013</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0E1LAzEQBuAgCtYq-BMqXqqwOpNsPvYoi19QEETPYTZNYGvbXZPtof_eSBXRi4chc3gyw7yMnSJcIQh9TQCCI5Z7bIRGm0LzCvZzj5UqcuEhO0ppAZBxiSN2Vncxeje03TpNhm7yTNQuVvTmY5pMeZ51ccwOAi2TP_l6x-z17valfihmT_eP9c2sIJRcFoaE9s41qAOKOejKKAWl5J4HL00VcA5YqmCchsAbwaESBEIa4o1TZZBizM53c_vYvW98Guyi28R1XmlRcJV_cPOPwlJDvkxnNd0pF7uUog-2j-2K4tYi2M-Y7HdMmV7uKPVk-7R1FIfWLX1ymxzMerDO91Zpi1ZWP9v_4l_qAyDzb_s</recordid><startdate>201303</startdate><enddate>201303</enddate><creator>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</creator><general>Educational Publishing Foundation</general><general>Canadian Psychological Association</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8FQ</scope><scope>8FV</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M3G</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201303</creationdate><title>Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)</title><author>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a1525-8a37eccb17f13d0798660452e2fe589f1d0146f8c70f2b32093a0358a2bc64f53</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2013</creationdate><topic>Experimental Design</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Statistical Analysis</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>PsycARTICLES</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Proquest)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Canadian Business &amp; Current Affairs Database (CBCA)</collection><collection>Canadian Business &amp; Current Affairs Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>eLibrary</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest research library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>CBCA Reference &amp; Current Events</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>Canadian journal of experimental psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W.</au><au>Mewhort, Douglas J. K</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)</atitle><jtitle>Canadian journal of experimental psychology</jtitle><date>2013-03</date><risdate>2013</risdate><volume>67</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>59</spage><epage>59</epage><pages>59-59</pages><issn>1196-1961</issn><eissn>1878-7290</eissn><coden>CJEPEK</coden><abstract>Reports an error in "A further look at the "language-as-fixed-effect fallacy"" by Jeroen G. W. Raaijmakers ( Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 2003[Sep], Vol 57[3], 141-151). The corrected version of Table 7 is listed below. The accompanying text on p. 147 should read: “For these data, F’ = 5.95 ( df= 1,7). Using either the statistics from Table 7 or by running separate subjects and item analyses, we obtain F 1= 1157.1 ( df= 1,3) and F₂= 5.97 ( df= 1,7). This leads to minF ’ 5.94 ( df =1,7).” The conclusions drawn from this example are not affected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2003-08374-003.) The proper analysis of experiments using language materials has been a source of controversy and debate among researchers. We summarize the main issues and discuss the solutions that have been presented. Even though the major issues have been dealt with extensively in the literature, there still exists quite a bit of confusion about how to analyze the data from such experiments. We discuss a number of the most frequently voiced objections. In particular, we discuss the issue of what happens if in a counterbalanced design only some of the items show the treatment effect. Finally, a possible solution is discussed for the case where only partial matching of items between conditions is possible. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved) (Source: journal abstract)</abstract><cop>Ottawa</cop><pub>Educational Publishing Foundation</pub><doi>10.1037/a0032114</doi><tpages>1</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1196-1961
ispartof Canadian journal of experimental psychology, 2013-03, Vol.67 (1), p.59-59
issn 1196-1961
1878-7290
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1326320285
source PsycARTICLES; Social Science Premium Collection
subjects Experimental Design
Language
Statistical Analysis
title Corrections to Raaijmakers (2003)
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-06T00%3A13%3A33IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Corrections%20to%20Raaijmakers%20(2003)&rft.jtitle=Canadian%20journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology&rft.au=Raaijmakers,%20Jeroen%20G.%20W.&rft.date=2013-03&rft.volume=67&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=59&rft.epage=59&rft.pages=59-59&rft.issn=1196-1961&rft.eissn=1878-7290&rft.coden=CJEPEK&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/a0032114&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1314700017%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a1525-8a37eccb17f13d0798660452e2fe589f1d0146f8c70f2b32093a0358a2bc64f53%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1314700017&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true