Loading…
Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference
Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response...
Saved in:
Published in: | The Psychological record 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43 |
container_end_page | 40 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 31 |
container_title | The Psychological record |
container_volume | 64 |
creator | Jiménez, Ángel Andrés Aparicio, Carlos F. |
description | Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1627083249</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3505382551</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE9LxDAQxYMouK5-AG8FrxtNMtm0PUrxHxQUWc8hTadul91kN2kFv71Z6sGLl5mBee_N8CPkmrNbzlh-FyXLQVDGJWWMCapOyExwBRTkUpySGWMAVJQA5-Qixk3S8OSYkbpa-95i1rtsFYyL_dB7t8iqtXGf6L8wZO8Y995FTMNh7APu0A1xkRnXZrW3Zpu9BewwoLN4Sc46s4149dvn5OPxYVU90_r16aW6r6kFrgbaNqIRpgUB1mADCgw0xvBlW5Yt5HmTg-lKpYq0kLYowCzxWKUpAErZSZiTmyl3H_xhxDjojR-DSyc1VyJnBQhZJhWfVDb4GNOTeh_6nQnfmjN9hKYnaDqh0EdoWiWPmDwxaROB8Cf5X9MPVF5uWA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1627083249</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Business Source Ultimate【Trial: -2024/12/31】【Remote access available】</source><source>Springer Nature</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Sociology Collection</source><creator>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</creator><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><description>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0033-2933</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2163-3452</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6</identifier><identifier>CODEN: PYRCAI</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cham: Springer International Publishing</publisher><subject>Alternatives ; Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Experiments ; Feedback (Response) ; Food ; Original Article ; Preferences ; Psychology ; Studies</subject><ispartof>The Psychological record, 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40</ispartof><rights>Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014</rights><rights>Copyright Association for Behavior Analysis International 2014</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1627083249?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,12846,21378,21394,21395,27924,27925,30999,33611,33877,34530,43733,43880,44115</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><title>The Psychological record</title><addtitle>Psychol Rec</addtitle><description>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</description><subject>Alternatives</subject><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>Original Article</subject><subject>Preferences</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><issn>0033-2933</issn><issn>2163-3452</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2014</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CJNVE</sourceid><sourceid>HEHIP</sourceid><sourceid>M0P</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><sourceid>M2S</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE9LxDAQxYMouK5-AG8FrxtNMtm0PUrxHxQUWc8hTadul91kN2kFv71Z6sGLl5mBee_N8CPkmrNbzlh-FyXLQVDGJWWMCapOyExwBRTkUpySGWMAVJQA5-Qixk3S8OSYkbpa-95i1rtsFYyL_dB7t8iqtXGf6L8wZO8Y995FTMNh7APu0A1xkRnXZrW3Zpu9BewwoLN4Sc46s4149dvn5OPxYVU90_r16aW6r6kFrgbaNqIRpgUB1mADCgw0xvBlW5Yt5HmTg-lKpYq0kLYowCzxWKUpAErZSZiTmyl3H_xhxDjojR-DSyc1VyJnBQhZJhWfVDb4GNOTeh_6nQnfmjN9hKYnaDqh0EdoWiWPmDwxaROB8Cf5X9MPVF5uWA</recordid><startdate>20140301</startdate><enddate>20140301</enddate><creator>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creator><creator>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creator><general>Springer International Publishing</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>HEHIP</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M2S</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20140301</creationdate><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><author>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2014</creationdate><topic>Alternatives</topic><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>Original Article</topic><topic>Preferences</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>Sociology Collection</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>ProQuest research library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Science Journals</collection><collection>ProQuest sociology</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>The Psychological record</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</au><au>Aparicio, Carlos F.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</atitle><jtitle>The Psychological record</jtitle><stitle>Psychol Rec</stitle><date>2014-03-01</date><risdate>2014</risdate><volume>64</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>31</spage><epage>40</epage><pages>31-40</pages><issn>0033-2933</issn><eissn>2163-3452</eissn><coden>PYRCAI</coden><abstract>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</abstract><cop>Cham</cop><pub>Springer International Publishing</pub><doi>10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0033-2933 |
ispartof | The Psychological record, 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40 |
issn | 0033-2933 2163-3452 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_1627083249 |
source | Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Business Source Ultimate【Trial: -2024/12/31】【Remote access available】; Springer Nature; Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Sociology Collection |
subjects | Alternatives Behavioral Science and Psychology Experiments Feedback (Response) Food Original Article Preferences Psychology Studies |
title | Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-01T14%3A39%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Choice%20in%20Transition,%20Changeover%20Response%20Requirements,%20and%20Local%20Preference&rft.jtitle=The%20Psychological%20record&rft.au=Jim%C3%A9nez,%20%C3%81ngel%20Andr%C3%A9s&rft.date=2014-03-01&rft.volume=64&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=31&rft.epage=40&rft.pages=31-40&rft.issn=0033-2933&rft.eissn=2163-3452&rft.coden=PYRCAI&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E3505382551%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1627083249&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |