Loading…

Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference

Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:The Psychological record 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40
Main Authors: Jiménez, Ángel Andrés, Aparicio, Carlos F.
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43
container_end_page 40
container_issue 1
container_start_page 31
container_title The Psychological record
container_volume 64
creator Jiménez, Ángel Andrés
Aparicio, Carlos F.
description Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1627083249</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3505382551</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE9LxDAQxYMouK5-AG8FrxtNMtm0PUrxHxQUWc8hTadul91kN2kFv71Z6sGLl5mBee_N8CPkmrNbzlh-FyXLQVDGJWWMCapOyExwBRTkUpySGWMAVJQA5-Qixk3S8OSYkbpa-95i1rtsFYyL_dB7t8iqtXGf6L8wZO8Y995FTMNh7APu0A1xkRnXZrW3Zpu9BewwoLN4Sc46s4149dvn5OPxYVU90_r16aW6r6kFrgbaNqIRpgUB1mADCgw0xvBlW5Yt5HmTg-lKpYq0kLYowCzxWKUpAErZSZiTmyl3H_xhxDjojR-DSyc1VyJnBQhZJhWfVDb4GNOTeh_6nQnfmjN9hKYnaDqh0EdoWiWPmDwxaROB8Cf5X9MPVF5uWA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1627083249</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Business Source Ultimate【Trial: -2024/12/31】【Remote access available】</source><source>Springer Nature</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Sociology Collection</source><creator>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</creator><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><description>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0033-2933</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2163-3452</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6</identifier><identifier>CODEN: PYRCAI</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cham: Springer International Publishing</publisher><subject>Alternatives ; Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Experiments ; Feedback (Response) ; Food ; Original Article ; Preferences ; Psychology ; Studies</subject><ispartof>The Psychological record, 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40</ispartof><rights>Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014</rights><rights>Copyright Association for Behavior Analysis International 2014</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1627083249?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,12846,21378,21394,21395,27924,27925,30999,33611,33877,34530,43733,43880,44115</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><title>The Psychological record</title><addtitle>Psychol Rec</addtitle><description>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</description><subject>Alternatives</subject><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>Original Article</subject><subject>Preferences</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><issn>0033-2933</issn><issn>2163-3452</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2014</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CJNVE</sourceid><sourceid>HEHIP</sourceid><sourceid>M0P</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><sourceid>M2S</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE9LxDAQxYMouK5-AG8FrxtNMtm0PUrxHxQUWc8hTadul91kN2kFv71Z6sGLl5mBee_N8CPkmrNbzlh-FyXLQVDGJWWMCapOyExwBRTkUpySGWMAVJQA5-Qixk3S8OSYkbpa-95i1rtsFYyL_dB7t8iqtXGf6L8wZO8Y995FTMNh7APu0A1xkRnXZrW3Zpu9BewwoLN4Sc46s4149dvn5OPxYVU90_r16aW6r6kFrgbaNqIRpgUB1mADCgw0xvBlW5Yt5HmTg-lKpYq0kLYowCzxWKUpAErZSZiTmyl3H_xhxDjojR-DSyc1VyJnBQhZJhWfVDb4GNOTeh_6nQnfmjN9hKYnaDqh0EdoWiWPmDwxaROB8Cf5X9MPVF5uWA</recordid><startdate>20140301</startdate><enddate>20140301</enddate><creator>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creator><creator>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creator><general>Springer International Publishing</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>HEHIP</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M2S</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20140301</creationdate><title>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</title><author>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés ; Aparicio, Carlos F.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2014</creationdate><topic>Alternatives</topic><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>Original Article</topic><topic>Preferences</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aparicio, Carlos F.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>Sociology Collection</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>ProQuest research library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Science Journals</collection><collection>ProQuest sociology</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><jtitle>The Psychological record</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Jiménez, Ángel Andrés</au><au>Aparicio, Carlos F.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference</atitle><jtitle>The Psychological record</jtitle><stitle>Psychol Rec</stitle><date>2014-03-01</date><risdate>2014</risdate><volume>64</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>31</spage><epage>40</epage><pages>31-40</pages><issn>0033-2933</issn><eissn>2163-3452</eissn><coden>PYRCAI</coden><abstract>Rats responded for food on concurrent random interval schedules arranging seven unsignaled food-rate ratios within sessions. Pressing a changeover lever allowed alternation between a high and a low probability of food lever. Switching from the former to the latter lever always required one response on the changeover lever. In successive conditions, switching from the low to the high probability of food lever required 1, 16, 32, or 48 responses. With successive food deliveries within components, sensitivity to food-rate ratio increased sooner with large than with the small changeover requirements. Preference became more extreme toward the just-productive lever following continuations of food deliveries. Discontinuations of food deliveries shifted preference toward the other lever. Following food deliveries, pulses of preference were of similar size across conditions. Yet, preference for the just-productive lever moved slowly toward indifference with the largest requirements. These findings support the notion that food deliveries exert effects on choice at extended and local time scales.</abstract><cop>Cham</cop><pub>Springer International Publishing</pub><doi>10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0033-2933
ispartof The Psychological record, 2014-03, Vol.64 (1), p.31-40
issn 0033-2933
2163-3452
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1627083249
source Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Education Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Business Source Ultimate【Trial: -2024/12/31】【Remote access available】; Springer Nature; Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Sociology Collection
subjects Alternatives
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Experiments
Feedback (Response)
Food
Original Article
Preferences
Psychology
Studies
title Choice in Transition, Changeover Response Requirements, and Local Preference
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-01T14%3A39%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Choice%20in%20Transition,%20Changeover%20Response%20Requirements,%20and%20Local%20Preference&rft.jtitle=The%20Psychological%20record&rft.au=Jim%C3%A9nez,%20%C3%81ngel%20Andr%C3%A9s&rft.date=2014-03-01&rft.volume=64&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=31&rft.epage=40&rft.pages=31-40&rft.issn=0033-2933&rft.eissn=2163-3452&rft.coden=PYRCAI&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s40732-014-0002-6&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E3505382551%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c316t-db2b2ad323caeb363a3baa15d99d377b73af966863a4c883a5e883a4a83394f43%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1627083249&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true