Loading…
Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)
It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple matin...
Saved in:
Published in: | Journal of comparative psychology (1983) 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295 |
---|---|
Main Author: | |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93 |
---|---|
cites | |
container_end_page | 295 |
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 291 |
container_title | Journal of comparative psychology (1983) |
container_volume | 100 |
creator | Verrell, Paul A |
description | It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt,
Triturus vulgaris vulgaris
(L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_614302313</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1295231150</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kEtLAzEUhYMoWKu_wE3wAQpOzWMyM1mW4guqLqxuQ5pJ2pSZzphklP57U1pKN25u7g3fOZd7ADjHaIARze9RTlmSI5rFEQ3ogHB8AHqYU54QVOSHoLcjjsGJ9wuEUIbTvAe-xra2QZfwVVY6lmCXMziSrVQ2rKBdwjDX8KNumjCHb_o33MGJs6FznYc_XTWTzu41N8O6nduplben4MjIyuuz7dsHn48Pk9FzMn5_ehkNx4kknIZEa6ZVllPDNCmIMpwrzkoiS4U5m6a6KDgryoxJgnmqdFrq-MVKg008qzCc9sHFxrd1zXenfRCLpnPLuFLE-ygiFNMIXf4HYcJZZDBDkaIbSrnGe6eNaJ2tpVsJjMQ6ZbHOUKwzjCMSVMSUo-pq6y29kpVxcqms30kLnEVBEbHrDRaTFa1fKemCVZX2QjX1nt0fo5qHsQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614302313</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><creator>Verrell, Paul A</creator><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><description>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt,
Triturus vulgaris vulgaris
(L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0735-7036</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-2087</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JCOPDT</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, DC: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Amphibia and reptilia ; Animal ; Animal ethology ; Animal Mating Behavior ; Biological and medical sciences ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Male ; Male Animals ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Salamanders ; Vertebrata</subject><ispartof>Journal of comparative psychology (1983), 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295</ispartof><rights>1986 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1987 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>1986, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=8167038$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><title>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</title><description>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt,
Triturus vulgaris vulgaris
(L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</description><subject>Amphibia and reptilia</subject><subject>Animal</subject><subject>Animal ethology</subject><subject>Animal Mating Behavior</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Male Animals</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Salamanders</subject><subject>Vertebrata</subject><issn>0735-7036</issn><issn>1939-2087</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1986</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp1kEtLAzEUhYMoWKu_wE3wAQpOzWMyM1mW4guqLqxuQ5pJ2pSZzphklP57U1pKN25u7g3fOZd7ADjHaIARze9RTlmSI5rFEQ3ogHB8AHqYU54QVOSHoLcjjsGJ9wuEUIbTvAe-xra2QZfwVVY6lmCXMziSrVQ2rKBdwjDX8KNumjCHb_o33MGJs6FznYc_XTWTzu41N8O6nduplben4MjIyuuz7dsHn48Pk9FzMn5_ehkNx4kknIZEa6ZVllPDNCmIMpwrzkoiS4U5m6a6KDgryoxJgnmqdFrq-MVKg008qzCc9sHFxrd1zXenfRCLpnPLuFLE-ygiFNMIXf4HYcJZZDBDkaIbSrnGe6eNaJ2tpVsJjMQ6ZbHOUKwzjCMSVMSUo-pq6y29kpVxcqms30kLnEVBEbHrDRaTFa1fKemCVZX2QjX1nt0fo5qHsQ</recordid><startdate>19860901</startdate><enddate>19860901</enddate><creator>Verrell, Paul A</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19860901</creationdate><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><author>Verrell, Paul A</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1986</creationdate><topic>Amphibia and reptilia</topic><topic>Animal</topic><topic>Animal ethology</topic><topic>Animal Mating Behavior</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Male Animals</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Salamanders</topic><topic>Vertebrata</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>PsycArticles</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><jtitle>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Verrell, Paul A</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</atitle><jtitle>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</jtitle><date>1986-09-01</date><risdate>1986</risdate><volume>100</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>291</spage><epage>295</epage><pages>291-295</pages><issn>0735-7036</issn><eissn>1939-2087</eissn><coden>JCOPDT</coden><abstract>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt,
Triturus vulgaris vulgaris
(L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</abstract><cop>Washington, DC</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291</doi><tpages>5</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0735-7036 |
ispartof | Journal of comparative psychology (1983), 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295 |
issn | 0735-7036 1939-2087 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_journals_614302313 |
source | EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES |
subjects | Amphibia and reptilia Animal Animal ethology Animal Mating Behavior Biological and medical sciences Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Male Male Animals Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Salamanders Vertebrata |
title | Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia) |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T02%3A13%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Limited%20Male%20Mating%20Capacity%20in%20the%20Smooth%20Newt,%20Triturus%20vulgaris%20vulgaris%20(Amphibia)&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20comparative%20psychology%20(1983)&rft.au=Verrell,%20Paul%20A&rft.date=1986-09-01&rft.volume=100&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=291&rft.epage=295&rft.pages=291-295&rft.issn=0735-7036&rft.eissn=1939-2087&rft.coden=JCOPDT&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1295231150%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614302313&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |