Loading…

Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)

It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple matin...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Journal of comparative psychology (1983) 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295
Main Author: Verrell, Paul A
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93
cites
container_end_page 295
container_issue 3
container_start_page 291
container_title Journal of comparative psychology (1983)
container_volume 100
creator Verrell, Paul A
description It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.
doi_str_mv 10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_614302313</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1295231150</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kEtLAzEUhYMoWKu_wE3wAQpOzWMyM1mW4guqLqxuQ5pJ2pSZzphklP57U1pKN25u7g3fOZd7ADjHaIARze9RTlmSI5rFEQ3ogHB8AHqYU54QVOSHoLcjjsGJ9wuEUIbTvAe-xra2QZfwVVY6lmCXMziSrVQ2rKBdwjDX8KNumjCHb_o33MGJs6FznYc_XTWTzu41N8O6nduplben4MjIyuuz7dsHn48Pk9FzMn5_ehkNx4kknIZEa6ZVllPDNCmIMpwrzkoiS4U5m6a6KDgryoxJgnmqdFrq-MVKg008qzCc9sHFxrd1zXenfRCLpnPLuFLE-ygiFNMIXf4HYcJZZDBDkaIbSrnGe6eNaJ2tpVsJjMQ6ZbHOUKwzjCMSVMSUo-pq6y29kpVxcqms30kLnEVBEbHrDRaTFa1fKemCVZX2QjX1nt0fo5qHsQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614302313</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><creator>Verrell, Paul A</creator><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><description>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0735-7036</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-2087</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JCOPDT</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, DC: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Amphibia and reptilia ; Animal ; Animal ethology ; Animal Mating Behavior ; Biological and medical sciences ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Male ; Male Animals ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Salamanders ; Vertebrata</subject><ispartof>Journal of comparative psychology (1983), 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295</ispartof><rights>1986 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1987 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>1986, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=8167038$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><title>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</title><description>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</description><subject>Amphibia and reptilia</subject><subject>Animal</subject><subject>Animal ethology</subject><subject>Animal Mating Behavior</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Male Animals</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Salamanders</subject><subject>Vertebrata</subject><issn>0735-7036</issn><issn>1939-2087</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1986</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp1kEtLAzEUhYMoWKu_wE3wAQpOzWMyM1mW4guqLqxuQ5pJ2pSZzphklP57U1pKN25u7g3fOZd7ADjHaIARze9RTlmSI5rFEQ3ogHB8AHqYU54QVOSHoLcjjsGJ9wuEUIbTvAe-xra2QZfwVVY6lmCXMziSrVQ2rKBdwjDX8KNumjCHb_o33MGJs6FznYc_XTWTzu41N8O6nduplben4MjIyuuz7dsHn48Pk9FzMn5_ehkNx4kknIZEa6ZVllPDNCmIMpwrzkoiS4U5m6a6KDgryoxJgnmqdFrq-MVKg008qzCc9sHFxrd1zXenfRCLpnPLuFLE-ygiFNMIXf4HYcJZZDBDkaIbSrnGe6eNaJ2tpVsJjMQ6ZbHOUKwzjCMSVMSUo-pq6y29kpVxcqms30kLnEVBEbHrDRaTFa1fKemCVZX2QjX1nt0fo5qHsQ</recordid><startdate>19860901</startdate><enddate>19860901</enddate><creator>Verrell, Paul A</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19860901</creationdate><title>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</title><author>Verrell, Paul A</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1986</creationdate><topic>Amphibia and reptilia</topic><topic>Animal</topic><topic>Animal ethology</topic><topic>Animal Mating Behavior</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Male Animals</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Salamanders</topic><topic>Vertebrata</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Verrell, Paul A</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>PsycArticles</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><jtitle>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Verrell, Paul A</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)</atitle><jtitle>Journal of comparative psychology (1983)</jtitle><date>1986-09-01</date><risdate>1986</risdate><volume>100</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>291</spage><epage>295</epage><pages>291-295</pages><issn>0735-7036</issn><eissn>1939-2087</eissn><coden>JCOPDT</coden><abstract>It is commonly assumed that male animals are able to produce large numbers of sperm and/or ejaculates at a rapid rate and that male mating success is thus constrained mainly by the availability of receptive females. Two experiments were conducted in order to determine the capacity for multiple mating in males of the European smooth newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (L.). When exposed to four potential mates simultaneously (Experiment 1), all males tested completed one courtship encounter, depositing at least one spermatophore each. The temporal structure of the courtship of males exposed to four females was not different from that of males courting in the absence of other females. The majority of the males tested were unable to complete a second courtship encounter that was initiated immediately following a first; only 9% of the males tested deposited spermatophores during the second encounter. In Experiment 2, males were allowed to "rest" between successive courtship encounters. A period of rest of between 24 and 48 hr was required if the same number of spermatophores was to be deposited during each courtship encounter. These results clearly indicate that male mating capacity is intrinsically limited in the smooth newt, and it is suggested that some aspect of spermatophore availability is responsible for this constraint.</abstract><cop>Washington, DC</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><doi>10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291</doi><tpages>5</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0735-7036
ispartof Journal of comparative psychology (1983), 1986-09, Vol.100 (3), p.291-295
issn 0735-7036
1939-2087
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_614302313
source EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES
subjects Amphibia and reptilia
Animal
Animal ethology
Animal Mating Behavior
Biological and medical sciences
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Male
Male Animals
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Salamanders
Vertebrata
title Limited Male Mating Capacity in the Smooth Newt, Triturus vulgaris vulgaris (Amphibia)
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T02%3A13%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Limited%20Male%20Mating%20Capacity%20in%20the%20Smooth%20Newt,%20Triturus%20vulgaris%20vulgaris%20(Amphibia)&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20comparative%20psychology%20(1983)&rft.au=Verrell,%20Paul%20A&rft.date=1986-09-01&rft.volume=100&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=291&rft.epage=295&rft.pages=291-295&rft.issn=0735-7036&rft.eissn=1939-2087&rft.coden=JCOPDT&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.291&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1295231150%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a293t-ee5ec673f5e282cf99c95d2adc195b4e88958d65a2194ce4dee885df1f2088f93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614302313&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true