Loading…

Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage

Movement times to the first target in a 2-target sequence are typically slower than in 1-target aiming tasks. The 1-target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge regardless of hand preference, the hand used, the amount of practice, and the availability of visual feedback. The authors teste...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Journal of motor behavior 2010-09, Vol.42 (5), p.325-330
Main Authors: Khan, Michael A., Mottram, Thomas M., Adam, Jos J., Buckolz, Eric
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03
container_end_page 330
container_issue 5
container_start_page 325
container_title Journal of motor behavior
container_volume 42
creator Khan, Michael A.
Mottram, Thomas M.
Adam, Jos J.
Buckolz, Eric
description Movement times to the first target in a 2-target sequence are typically slower than in 1-target aiming tasks. The 1-target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge regardless of hand preference, the hand used, the amount of practice, and the availability of visual feedback. The authors tested central and peripheral explanations of the 1-target advantage, as postulated by the movement integration hypothesis, by asking participants to perform single-target movements, 2-target movements with 1 limb, and 2-target movements in which they switched limbs at the first target. Reaction time and movement time data showed a 1-target advantage that was similar for both 1- and 2-limb sequential aiming movements. This outcome demonstrates that the processes underlying the increase in movement time to the 1st target in 2-target sequences are not specific to the limb, suggesting that the 1-target advantage originates at a central rather than a peripheral level.
doi_str_mv 10.1080/00222895.2010.510544
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_759263888</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>888119122</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU1P4zAQhq0VaCmw_2C1irhwCowndpMcVqiq-JIqcaCcrYnjFKPEYe2Uin-Po7Z74ABzGWn0zDsfL2O_OVxwKOASABGLUl4gxJLkIIX4wSa8FJBywPyATUYkHZkjdhzCC8TIAX6yI4QCpwLFhP19NP_Wxg2W2mRmO-tWycYOz8ly0ycL21UhIVcnw7NJHpxJl-RXZkhm9Ru5gVbmlB021Abza5dP2NPN9XJ-ly4ebu_ns0WqBfIhNUIL0rrMa46AQkCGVMlCapkT1YCFqLTWsirJNKIysiaZGSkak_FKT2vITtj5VvfV93HdMKjOBm3alpzp10EVRcF5yRG_JXMp4jwUo-bZJ_KlX3sXz4hQidMsikZIbCHt-xC8adSrtx35d8VBjTaovQ1qtEFtbYhtf3ba66oz9f-m_d8jcLUFrGt639Gm922tBnpve994ctoGlX054gMrL5RB</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>759263888</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage</title><source>EBSCOhost SPORTDiscus with Full Text</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Education Collection</source><source>Taylor and Francis Social Sciences and Humanities Collection</source><creator>Khan, Michael A. ; Mottram, Thomas M. ; Adam, Jos J. ; Buckolz, Eric</creator><creatorcontrib>Khan, Michael A. ; Mottram, Thomas M. ; Adam, Jos J. ; Buckolz, Eric</creatorcontrib><description>Movement times to the first target in a 2-target sequence are typically slower than in 1-target aiming tasks. The 1-target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge regardless of hand preference, the hand used, the amount of practice, and the availability of visual feedback. The authors tested central and peripheral explanations of the 1-target advantage, as postulated by the movement integration hypothesis, by asking participants to perform single-target movements, 2-target movements with 1 limb, and 2-target movements in which they switched limbs at the first target. Reaction time and movement time data showed a 1-target advantage that was similar for both 1- and 2-limb sequential aiming movements. This outcome demonstrates that the processes underlying the increase in movement time to the 1st target in 2-target sequences are not specific to the limb, suggesting that the 1-target advantage originates at a central rather than a peripheral level.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-2895</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1940-1027</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2010.510544</identifier><identifier>PMID: 20826424</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JMTBAB</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Taylor &amp; Francis Group</publisher><subject>Accuracy ; Adolescent ; Adult ; Extremities - physiology ; Feedback (Response) ; Female ; Handedness ; Humans ; Male ; Motor ability ; Movement ; online control processes ; Programming ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; rapid aiming movements ; Reaction Time ; response complexity</subject><ispartof>Journal of motor behavior, 2010-09, Vol.42 (5), p.325-330</ispartof><rights>Copyright Taylor &amp; Francis Group, LLC 2010</rights><rights>Copyright Taylor &amp; Francis Inc. Sep/Oct 2010</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/759263888/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/759263888?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21378,21394,27924,27925,33611,33612,33877,33878,43733,43880,74221,74397</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20826424$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Khan, Michael A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mottram, Thomas M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Adam, Jos J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Buckolz, Eric</creatorcontrib><title>Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage</title><title>Journal of motor behavior</title><addtitle>J Mot Behav</addtitle><description>Movement times to the first target in a 2-target sequence are typically slower than in 1-target aiming tasks. The 1-target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge regardless of hand preference, the hand used, the amount of practice, and the availability of visual feedback. The authors tested central and peripheral explanations of the 1-target advantage, as postulated by the movement integration hypothesis, by asking participants to perform single-target movements, 2-target movements with 1 limb, and 2-target movements in which they switched limbs at the first target. Reaction time and movement time data showed a 1-target advantage that was similar for both 1- and 2-limb sequential aiming movements. This outcome demonstrates that the processes underlying the increase in movement time to the 1st target in 2-target sequences are not specific to the limb, suggesting that the 1-target advantage originates at a central rather than a peripheral level.</description><subject>Accuracy</subject><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Extremities - physiology</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Handedness</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Motor ability</subject><subject>Movement</subject><subject>online control processes</subject><subject>Programming</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>rapid aiming movements</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>response complexity</subject><issn>0022-2895</issn><issn>1940-1027</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2010</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CJNVE</sourceid><sourceid>M0P</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU1P4zAQhq0VaCmw_2C1irhwCowndpMcVqiq-JIqcaCcrYnjFKPEYe2Uin-Po7Z74ABzGWn0zDsfL2O_OVxwKOASABGLUl4gxJLkIIX4wSa8FJBywPyATUYkHZkjdhzCC8TIAX6yI4QCpwLFhP19NP_Wxg2W2mRmO-tWycYOz8ly0ycL21UhIVcnw7NJHpxJl-RXZkhm9Ru5gVbmlB021Abza5dP2NPN9XJ-ly4ebu_ns0WqBfIhNUIL0rrMa46AQkCGVMlCapkT1YCFqLTWsirJNKIysiaZGSkak_FKT2vITtj5VvfV93HdMKjOBm3alpzp10EVRcF5yRG_JXMp4jwUo-bZJ_KlX3sXz4hQidMsikZIbCHt-xC8adSrtx35d8VBjTaovQ1qtEFtbYhtf3ba66oz9f-m_d8jcLUFrGt639Gm922tBnpve994ctoGlX054gMrL5RB</recordid><startdate>20100901</startdate><enddate>20100901</enddate><creator>Khan, Michael A.</creator><creator>Mottram, Thomas M.</creator><creator>Adam, Jos J.</creator><creator>Buckolz, Eric</creator><general>Taylor &amp; Francis Group</general><general>Taylor &amp; Francis Inc</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9-</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M0R</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20100901</creationdate><title>Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage</title><author>Khan, Michael A. ; Mottram, Thomas M. ; Adam, Jos J. ; Buckolz, Eric</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2010</creationdate><topic>Accuracy</topic><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Extremities - physiology</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Handedness</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Motor ability</topic><topic>Movement</topic><topic>online control processes</topic><topic>Programming</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>rapid aiming movements</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>response complexity</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Khan, Michael A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mottram, Thomas M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Adam, Jos J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Buckolz, Eric</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Nursing and Allied Health Journals</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>Consumer Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Family Health Database (Proquest)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>PML(ProQuest Medical Library)</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Journal of motor behavior</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Khan, Michael A.</au><au>Mottram, Thomas M.</au><au>Adam, Jos J.</au><au>Buckolz, Eric</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage</atitle><jtitle>Journal of motor behavior</jtitle><addtitle>J Mot Behav</addtitle><date>2010-09-01</date><risdate>2010</risdate><volume>42</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>325</spage><epage>330</epage><pages>325-330</pages><issn>0022-2895</issn><eissn>1940-1027</eissn><coden>JMTBAB</coden><abstract>Movement times to the first target in a 2-target sequence are typically slower than in 1-target aiming tasks. The 1-target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge regardless of hand preference, the hand used, the amount of practice, and the availability of visual feedback. The authors tested central and peripheral explanations of the 1-target advantage, as postulated by the movement integration hypothesis, by asking participants to perform single-target movements, 2-target movements with 1 limb, and 2-target movements in which they switched limbs at the first target. Reaction time and movement time data showed a 1-target advantage that was similar for both 1- and 2-limb sequential aiming movements. This outcome demonstrates that the processes underlying the increase in movement time to the 1st target in 2-target sequences are not specific to the limb, suggesting that the 1-target advantage originates at a central rather than a peripheral level.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Taylor &amp; Francis Group</pub><pmid>20826424</pmid><doi>10.1080/00222895.2010.510544</doi><tpages>6</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0022-2895
ispartof Journal of motor behavior, 2010-09, Vol.42 (5), p.325-330
issn 0022-2895
1940-1027
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_759263888
source EBSCOhost SPORTDiscus with Full Text; Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Education Collection; Taylor and Francis Social Sciences and Humanities Collection
subjects Accuracy
Adolescent
Adult
Extremities - physiology
Feedback (Response)
Female
Handedness
Humans
Male
Motor ability
Movement
online control processes
Programming
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
rapid aiming movements
Reaction Time
response complexity
title Sequential Aiming with Two Limbs and the One-Target Advantage
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-20T08%3A23%3A52IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sequential%20Aiming%20with%20Two%20Limbs%20and%20the%20One-Target%20Advantage&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20motor%20behavior&rft.au=Khan,%20Michael%20A.&rft.date=2010-09-01&rft.volume=42&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=325&rft.epage=330&rft.pages=325-330&rft.issn=0022-2895&rft.eissn=1940-1027&rft.coden=JMTBAB&rft_id=info:doi/10.1080/00222895.2010.510544&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E888119122%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c421t-e4c4acc97d120244032ab585c57aad0284bccc5b9aef4be5da53e54fe31bc6d03%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=759263888&rft_id=info:pmid/20826424&rfr_iscdi=true