Loading…
Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm
Movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements in choice reaction time conditions, even when movements are cued directly by illuminating the targets (Blinch et al. in Exp Brain Res 232(3):947–955, 2014 ). This bimanual asymmetric cost may be caused...
Saved in:
Published in: | Experimental brain research 2015-06, Vol.233 (6), p.1931-1944 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3 |
container_end_page | 1944 |
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | 1931 |
container_title | Experimental brain research |
container_volume | 233 |
creator | Blinch, Jarrod Franks, Ian M. Carpenter, Mark G. Chua, Romeo |
description | Movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements in choice reaction time conditions, even when movements are cued directly by illuminating the targets (Blinch et al. in Exp Brain Res 232(3):947–955,
2014
). This bimanual asymmetric cost may be caused by increased processing demands on response programming, but this requires further investigation. The present experiment tested the demands on response programming for bimanual movements by temporally separating the preparation of each arm. This was achieved by precuing the target of one arm before the imperative stimulus. We asked: What was prepared in advance when one arm was precued? The answer to this question would suggest which process causes the bimanual asymmetric cost. Advance movement preparation was examined by comparing reaction times with and without a precue for the left target and by occasionally replacing the imperative stimulus with a loud, startling tone (120 dB). A startle tone releases whatever movement is prepared in advance with a much shorter reaction time than control trials (Carlsen et al. in Clin Neurophysiol 123(1):21–33,
2012
). Participants made bimanual symmetric and asymmetric reaching movements in simple and 2-choice reaction time conditions and a condition with a precue for the left target. We found a bimanual asymmetric cost in 2-choice conditions, and the asymmetric cost was significantly smaller when the left target was precued. These results, and the results from startle trials, suggest (1) that the precued movement was not fully programmed but partially programmed before the imperative stimulus and (2) that the asymmetric cost was caused by increased processing demands on response programming. Overall, the results support the notion that bimanual movements are not the sum of two unimanual movements; instead, the two arms of a bimanual movement are unified into a functional unit. When one target is precued, this critical unification likely occurs during response programming. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00221-015-4266-0 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1682423962</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3688964411</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kMtKxDAUhoMoznh5ADcScOOmenJp2i5FvMGAG8VlSNNT7dCmY9IOzNubMqOIIATCId__n_ARcsbgigFk1wGAc5YASxPJlUpgj8yZFDxhDNQ-mQMwmcicFTNyFMJyGkUGh2TG0zwFCWpO3l5dUzdYUWeG0SPta1o2nXGjaWnXr7FDNwTqcY2mjVS5oQFXxpuhce90-EC68ru5d1MYjf2gxncn5KA2bcDT3X1MXu_vXm4fk8Xzw9PtzSKxUvIhYULk3NjUCpmKsrLGVkoClyqtMB5esrKqKou5kRHKWJ0XpVWm4BlCJmUtjsnltnfl-88Rw6C7JlhsW-OwH4NmKueSi0LxiF78QZf96F383USxXBapEpFiW8r6PgSPtV75KMRvNAM9Wddb6zpa15N1DTFzvmseyw6rn8S35gjwLRDik3tH_2v1v61f4DiM7Q</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1681849563</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm</title><source>Springer Nature</source><source>ProQuest Social Science Premium Collection</source><creator>Blinch, Jarrod ; Franks, Ian M. ; Carpenter, Mark G. ; Chua, Romeo</creator><creatorcontrib>Blinch, Jarrod ; Franks, Ian M. ; Carpenter, Mark G. ; Chua, Romeo</creatorcontrib><description>Movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements in choice reaction time conditions, even when movements are cued directly by illuminating the targets (Blinch et al. in Exp Brain Res 232(3):947–955,
2014
). This bimanual asymmetric cost may be caused by increased processing demands on response programming, but this requires further investigation. The present experiment tested the demands on response programming for bimanual movements by temporally separating the preparation of each arm. This was achieved by precuing the target of one arm before the imperative stimulus. We asked: What was prepared in advance when one arm was precued? The answer to this question would suggest which process causes the bimanual asymmetric cost. Advance movement preparation was examined by comparing reaction times with and without a precue for the left target and by occasionally replacing the imperative stimulus with a loud, startling tone (120 dB). A startle tone releases whatever movement is prepared in advance with a much shorter reaction time than control trials (Carlsen et al. in Clin Neurophysiol 123(1):21–33,
2012
). Participants made bimanual symmetric and asymmetric reaching movements in simple and 2-choice reaction time conditions and a condition with a precue for the left target. We found a bimanual asymmetric cost in 2-choice conditions, and the asymmetric cost was significantly smaller when the left target was precued. These results, and the results from startle trials, suggest (1) that the precued movement was not fully programmed but partially programmed before the imperative stimulus and (2) that the asymmetric cost was caused by increased processing demands on response programming. Overall, the results support the notion that bimanual movements are not the sum of two unimanual movements; instead, the two arms of a bimanual movement are unified into a functional unit. When one target is precued, this critical unification likely occurs during response programming.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-015-4266-0</identifier><identifier>PMID: 25850406</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Acoustic Stimulation ; Adult ; Arm - physiology ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain research ; Choice Behavior - physiology ; Cues ; Discrimination ; Electromyography ; Evoked Potentials, Motor ; Female ; Functional Laterality - physiology ; Humans ; Male ; Movement - physiology ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Photic Stimulation ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time ; Reflex, Startle - physiology ; Research Article ; Visual Perception ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2015-06, Vol.233 (6), p.1931-1944</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1681849563/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1681849563?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21394,27924,27925,33611,33612,43733,74221</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25850406$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Blinch, Jarrod</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franks, Ian M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carpenter, Mark G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chua, Romeo</creatorcontrib><title>Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>Movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements in choice reaction time conditions, even when movements are cued directly by illuminating the targets (Blinch et al. in Exp Brain Res 232(3):947–955,
2014
). This bimanual asymmetric cost may be caused by increased processing demands on response programming, but this requires further investigation. The present experiment tested the demands on response programming for bimanual movements by temporally separating the preparation of each arm. This was achieved by precuing the target of one arm before the imperative stimulus. We asked: What was prepared in advance when one arm was precued? The answer to this question would suggest which process causes the bimanual asymmetric cost. Advance movement preparation was examined by comparing reaction times with and without a precue for the left target and by occasionally replacing the imperative stimulus with a loud, startling tone (120 dB). A startle tone releases whatever movement is prepared in advance with a much shorter reaction time than control trials (Carlsen et al. in Clin Neurophysiol 123(1):21–33,
2012
). Participants made bimanual symmetric and asymmetric reaching movements in simple and 2-choice reaction time conditions and a condition with a precue for the left target. We found a bimanual asymmetric cost in 2-choice conditions, and the asymmetric cost was significantly smaller when the left target was precued. These results, and the results from startle trials, suggest (1) that the precued movement was not fully programmed but partially programmed before the imperative stimulus and (2) that the asymmetric cost was caused by increased processing demands on response programming. Overall, the results support the notion that bimanual movements are not the sum of two unimanual movements; instead, the two arms of a bimanual movement are unified into a functional unit. When one target is precued, this critical unification likely occurs during response programming.</description><subject>Acoustic Stimulation</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Arm - physiology</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain research</subject><subject>Choice Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Discrimination</subject><subject>Electromyography</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials, Motor</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Functional Laterality - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Movement - physiology</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Reflex, Startle - physiology</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><subject>Visual Perception</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kMtKxDAUhoMoznh5ADcScOOmenJp2i5FvMGAG8VlSNNT7dCmY9IOzNubMqOIIATCId__n_ARcsbgigFk1wGAc5YASxPJlUpgj8yZFDxhDNQ-mQMwmcicFTNyFMJyGkUGh2TG0zwFCWpO3l5dUzdYUWeG0SPta1o2nXGjaWnXr7FDNwTqcY2mjVS5oQFXxpuhce90-EC68ru5d1MYjf2gxncn5KA2bcDT3X1MXu_vXm4fk8Xzw9PtzSKxUvIhYULk3NjUCpmKsrLGVkoClyqtMB5esrKqKou5kRHKWJ0XpVWm4BlCJmUtjsnltnfl-88Rw6C7JlhsW-OwH4NmKueSi0LxiF78QZf96F383USxXBapEpFiW8r6PgSPtV75KMRvNAM9Wddb6zpa15N1DTFzvmseyw6rn8S35gjwLRDik3tH_2v1v61f4DiM7Q</recordid><startdate>20150601</startdate><enddate>20150601</enddate><creator>Blinch, Jarrod</creator><creator>Franks, Ian M.</creator><creator>Carpenter, Mark G.</creator><creator>Chua, Romeo</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20150601</creationdate><title>Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm</title><author>Blinch, Jarrod ; Franks, Ian M. ; Carpenter, Mark G. ; Chua, Romeo</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Acoustic Stimulation</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Arm - physiology</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain research</topic><topic>Choice Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Discrimination</topic><topic>Electromyography</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials, Motor</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Functional Laterality - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Movement - physiology</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Reflex, Startle - physiology</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><topic>Visual Perception</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Blinch, Jarrod</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franks, Ian M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carpenter, Mark G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chua, Romeo</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health Medical collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology Journals</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Science Journals</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Blinch, Jarrod</au><au>Franks, Ian M.</au><au>Carpenter, Mark G.</au><au>Chua, Romeo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2015-06-01</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>233</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>1931</spage><epage>1944</epage><pages>1931-1944</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><abstract>Movement preparation of bimanual asymmetric movements is longer than bimanual symmetric movements in choice reaction time conditions, even when movements are cued directly by illuminating the targets (Blinch et al. in Exp Brain Res 232(3):947–955,
2014
). This bimanual asymmetric cost may be caused by increased processing demands on response programming, but this requires further investigation. The present experiment tested the demands on response programming for bimanual movements by temporally separating the preparation of each arm. This was achieved by precuing the target of one arm before the imperative stimulus. We asked: What was prepared in advance when one arm was precued? The answer to this question would suggest which process causes the bimanual asymmetric cost. Advance movement preparation was examined by comparing reaction times with and without a precue for the left target and by occasionally replacing the imperative stimulus with a loud, startling tone (120 dB). A startle tone releases whatever movement is prepared in advance with a much shorter reaction time than control trials (Carlsen et al. in Clin Neurophysiol 123(1):21–33,
2012
). Participants made bimanual symmetric and asymmetric reaching movements in simple and 2-choice reaction time conditions and a condition with a precue for the left target. We found a bimanual asymmetric cost in 2-choice conditions, and the asymmetric cost was significantly smaller when the left target was precued. These results, and the results from startle trials, suggest (1) that the precued movement was not fully programmed but partially programmed before the imperative stimulus and (2) that the asymmetric cost was caused by increased processing demands on response programming. Overall, the results support the notion that bimanual movements are not the sum of two unimanual movements; instead, the two arms of a bimanual movement are unified into a functional unit. When one target is precued, this critical unification likely occurs during response programming.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>25850406</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-015-4266-0</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0014-4819 |
ispartof | Experimental brain research, 2015-06, Vol.233 (6), p.1931-1944 |
issn | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1682423962 |
source | Springer Nature; ProQuest Social Science Premium Collection |
subjects | Acoustic Stimulation Adult Arm - physiology Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Brain research Choice Behavior - physiology Cues Discrimination Electromyography Evoked Potentials, Motor Female Functional Laterality - physiology Humans Male Movement - physiology Neurology Neurosciences Photic Stimulation Psychomotor Performance - physiology Reaction Time Reflex, Startle - physiology Research Article Visual Perception Young Adult |
title | Unified nature of bimanual movements revealed by separating the preparation of each arm |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-02T09%3A30%3A29IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Unified%20nature%20of%20bimanual%20movements%20revealed%20by%20separating%20the%20preparation%20of%20each%20arm&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=Blinch,%20Jarrod&rft.date=2015-06-01&rft.volume=233&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=1931&rft.epage=1944&rft.pages=1931-1944&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-015-4266-0&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E3688964411%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c442t-13382ac5c3453bdcacd6402465de5de2b1bdddce8a4c5c71f89bc6a927e0744f3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1681849563&rft_id=info:pmid/25850406&rfr_iscdi=true |