Loading…
Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics
Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that...
Saved in:
Published in: | Psychological science in the public interest 2007-11, Vol.8 (2), p.53-96 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523 |
container_end_page | 96 |
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 53 |
container_title | Psychological science in the public interest |
container_volume | 8 |
creator | Gigerenzer, Gerd Gaissmaier, Wolfgang Kurz-Milcke, Elke Schwartz, Lisa M. Woloshin, Steven |
description | Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that higher survival rates with cancer screening do not imply longer life, or that the statement that mammography screening reduces the risk of dying from breast cancer by 25% in fact means that 1 less woman out of 1,000 will die of the disease. We provide evidence that statistical illiteracy (a) is common to patients, journalists, and physicians; (b) is created by nontransparent framing of information that is sometimes an unintentional result of lack of understanding but can also be a result of intentional efforts to manipulate or persuade people; and (c) can have serious consequences for health. The causes of statistical illiteracy should not be attributed to cognitive biases alone, but to the emotional nature of the doctor-patient relationship and conflicts of interest in the healthcare system. The classic doctor-patient relation is based on (the physician's) paternalism and (the patient's) trust in authority, which make statistical literacy seem unnecessary; so does the traditional combination of determinism (physicians who seek causes, not chances) and the illusion of certainty (patients who seek certainty when there is none). We show that information pamphlets, Web sites, leaflets distributed to doctors by the pharma- ceutical industry, and even medical journals often report evidence in nontransparent forms that suggest big benefits of featured interventions and small harms. Without understanding the numbers involved, the public is susceptible to political and commercial manipulation of their anxieties and hopes, which undermines the goals of informed consent and shared decision making. What can be done? We discuss the importance of teaching statistical thinking and transparent representations in primary and secondary education as well as in medical school. Yet this requires familiarizing children early on with the concept of probability and teaching statistical literacy as the art of solving real-world problems rather than applying formulas to toy problems about coins and dice. A major precondition for statistical literacy is transparent risk communication. We recommend using frequency statements instead of single-event probabilities, absolute risks instead o |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_19618730</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>40062369</jstor_id><sage_id>10.1111_j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x</sage_id><sourcerecordid>40062369</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUtP4zAQgC3ECsrjJ4AsISEuCTN-1Za4IB7blViBVDhbbuJAQpqUOJXKv19nW9CKw-KL5fE3M_Z8hFCEFOM6r1KU3CQKJE8ZgE4BgPN0tUVGDBUk8YTbZISSmQQB1C7ZC6ECQIFS7ZBdplDhWJgRuZj4elE2z_S6zfq2C9Q1OX1wfembPtDf7tXTqW-Cp21BJ97V_Qud9vE69GUWDsiPwtXBH272ffJ0e_N4NUnu7n_-urq8SzIx5qsEhdR8Bi7n4JgsuHMZU9J4p53xXDEQAj3o8Wx4FosRgarIOcszrUFKxvfJ6bruomvflj70dl6GzNe1a3y7DBaNQj3mEMGz_4MqkiZOzUT05Atatcuuid-wqDXTyhguIqXXVNa1IXS-sIuunLvu3SLYQYWt7KDCDirsoML-VWFXMfV402A5m_v8M_Fj9hGQayC4Z_9P9-8LH63zqhCNfdYV0TPjyvA_6x6a-g</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1882869934</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Business Source Ultimate</source><source>JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection</source><source>Sage Journals Online</source><creator>Gigerenzer, Gerd ; Gaissmaier, Wolfgang ; Kurz-Milcke, Elke ; Schwartz, Lisa M. ; Woloshin, Steven</creator><creatorcontrib>Gigerenzer, Gerd ; Gaissmaier, Wolfgang ; Kurz-Milcke, Elke ; Schwartz, Lisa M. ; Woloshin, Steven</creatorcontrib><description>Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that higher survival rates with cancer screening do not imply longer life, or that the statement that mammography screening reduces the risk of dying from breast cancer by 25% in fact means that 1 less woman out of 1,000 will die of the disease. We provide evidence that statistical illiteracy (a) is common to patients, journalists, and physicians; (b) is created by nontransparent framing of information that is sometimes an unintentional result of lack of understanding but can also be a result of intentional efforts to manipulate or persuade people; and (c) can have serious consequences for health. The causes of statistical illiteracy should not be attributed to cognitive biases alone, but to the emotional nature of the doctor-patient relationship and conflicts of interest in the healthcare system. The classic doctor-patient relation is based on (the physician's) paternalism and (the patient's) trust in authority, which make statistical literacy seem unnecessary; so does the traditional combination of determinism (physicians who seek causes, not chances) and the illusion of certainty (patients who seek certainty when there is none). We show that information pamphlets, Web sites, leaflets distributed to doctors by the pharma- ceutical industry, and even medical journals often report evidence in nontransparent forms that suggest big benefits of featured interventions and small harms. Without understanding the numbers involved, the public is susceptible to political and commercial manipulation of their anxieties and hopes, which undermines the goals of informed consent and shared decision making. What can be done? We discuss the importance of teaching statistical thinking and transparent representations in primary and secondary education as well as in medical school. Yet this requires familiarizing children early on with the concept of probability and teaching statistical literacy as the art of solving real-world problems rather than applying formulas to toy problems about coins and dice. A major precondition for statistical literacy is transparent risk communication. We recommend using frequency statements instead of single-event probabilities, absolute risks instead of relative risks, mortality rates instead of survival rates, and natural frequencies instead of conditional probabilities. Psychological research on transparent visual and numerical forms of risk communication, as well as training of physicians in their use, is called for. Statistical literacy is a necessary precondition for an educated citizenship in a technological democracy. Understanding risks and asking critical questions can also shape the emotional climate in a society so that hopes and anxieties are no longer as easily manipulated from outside and citizens can develop a better-informed and more relaxed attitude toward their health.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1529-1006</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2160-0031</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1539-6053</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x</identifier><identifier>PMID: 26161749</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Los Angeles, CA: Blackwell Publishers</publisher><subject>Breast cancer ; Cancer screening ; Citizenship ; Cognitive bias ; Conflicts of interest ; Democracy ; Determinism ; Frame analysis ; Group decision making ; Health care ; Health care statistics ; Illiteracy ; Informed consent ; Journalists ; Leaflets ; Literacy ; Mammography ; Manipulation ; Meaning ; Medical journals ; Medical schools ; Medical screening ; Mortality ; Natural frequencies ; Paternalism ; Pharmaceutical industry ; Physician patient relationships ; Physicians ; Politicians ; Probabilities ; Statistics ; World problems</subject><ispartof>Psychological science in the public interest, 2007-11, Vol.8 (2), p.53-96</ispartof><rights>Copyright 2008 Association for Psychological Science</rights><rights>2008 Association for Psychological Science</rights><rights>2008 Association for Psychological Science.</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/40062369$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/40062369$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27903,27904,30978,58216,58449,79110</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26161749$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Gigerenzer, Gerd</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gaissmaier, Wolfgang</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kurz-Milcke, Elke</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schwartz, Lisa M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Woloshin, Steven</creatorcontrib><title>Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics</title><title>Psychological science in the public interest</title><addtitle>Psychol Sci Public Interest</addtitle><description>Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that higher survival rates with cancer screening do not imply longer life, or that the statement that mammography screening reduces the risk of dying from breast cancer by 25% in fact means that 1 less woman out of 1,000 will die of the disease. We provide evidence that statistical illiteracy (a) is common to patients, journalists, and physicians; (b) is created by nontransparent framing of information that is sometimes an unintentional result of lack of understanding but can also be a result of intentional efforts to manipulate or persuade people; and (c) can have serious consequences for health. The causes of statistical illiteracy should not be attributed to cognitive biases alone, but to the emotional nature of the doctor-patient relationship and conflicts of interest in the healthcare system. The classic doctor-patient relation is based on (the physician's) paternalism and (the patient's) trust in authority, which make statistical literacy seem unnecessary; so does the traditional combination of determinism (physicians who seek causes, not chances) and the illusion of certainty (patients who seek certainty when there is none). We show that information pamphlets, Web sites, leaflets distributed to doctors by the pharma- ceutical industry, and even medical journals often report evidence in nontransparent forms that suggest big benefits of featured interventions and small harms. Without understanding the numbers involved, the public is susceptible to political and commercial manipulation of their anxieties and hopes, which undermines the goals of informed consent and shared decision making. What can be done? We discuss the importance of teaching statistical thinking and transparent representations in primary and secondary education as well as in medical school. Yet this requires familiarizing children early on with the concept of probability and teaching statistical literacy as the art of solving real-world problems rather than applying formulas to toy problems about coins and dice. A major precondition for statistical literacy is transparent risk communication. We recommend using frequency statements instead of single-event probabilities, absolute risks instead of relative risks, mortality rates instead of survival rates, and natural frequencies instead of conditional probabilities. Psychological research on transparent visual and numerical forms of risk communication, as well as training of physicians in their use, is called for. Statistical literacy is a necessary precondition for an educated citizenship in a technological democracy. Understanding risks and asking critical questions can also shape the emotional climate in a society so that hopes and anxieties are no longer as easily manipulated from outside and citizens can develop a better-informed and more relaxed attitude toward their health.</description><subject>Breast cancer</subject><subject>Cancer screening</subject><subject>Citizenship</subject><subject>Cognitive bias</subject><subject>Conflicts of interest</subject><subject>Democracy</subject><subject>Determinism</subject><subject>Frame analysis</subject><subject>Group decision making</subject><subject>Health care</subject><subject>Health care statistics</subject><subject>Illiteracy</subject><subject>Informed consent</subject><subject>Journalists</subject><subject>Leaflets</subject><subject>Literacy</subject><subject>Mammography</subject><subject>Manipulation</subject><subject>Meaning</subject><subject>Medical journals</subject><subject>Medical schools</subject><subject>Medical screening</subject><subject>Mortality</subject><subject>Natural frequencies</subject><subject>Paternalism</subject><subject>Pharmaceutical industry</subject><subject>Physician patient relationships</subject><subject>Physicians</subject><subject>Politicians</subject><subject>Probabilities</subject><subject>Statistics</subject><subject>World problems</subject><issn>1529-1006</issn><issn>2160-0031</issn><issn>1539-6053</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2007</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkUtP4zAQgC3ECsrjJ4AsISEuCTN-1Za4IB7blViBVDhbbuJAQpqUOJXKv19nW9CKw-KL5fE3M_Z8hFCEFOM6r1KU3CQKJE8ZgE4BgPN0tUVGDBUk8YTbZISSmQQB1C7ZC6ECQIFS7ZBdplDhWJgRuZj4elE2z_S6zfq2C9Q1OX1wfembPtDf7tXTqW-Cp21BJ97V_Qud9vE69GUWDsiPwtXBH272ffJ0e_N4NUnu7n_-urq8SzIx5qsEhdR8Bi7n4JgsuHMZU9J4p53xXDEQAj3o8Wx4FosRgarIOcszrUFKxvfJ6bruomvflj70dl6GzNe1a3y7DBaNQj3mEMGz_4MqkiZOzUT05Atatcuuid-wqDXTyhguIqXXVNa1IXS-sIuunLvu3SLYQYWt7KDCDirsoML-VWFXMfV402A5m_v8M_Fj9hGQayC4Z_9P9-8LH63zqhCNfdYV0TPjyvA_6x6a-g</recordid><startdate>20071101</startdate><enddate>20071101</enddate><creator>Gigerenzer, Gerd</creator><creator>Gaissmaier, Wolfgang</creator><creator>Kurz-Milcke, Elke</creator><creator>Schwartz, Lisa M.</creator><creator>Woloshin, Steven</creator><general>Blackwell Publishers</general><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>SAGE PUBLICATIONS, INC</general><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7U1</scope><scope>7U2</scope><scope>C1K</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20071101</creationdate><title>Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics</title><author>Gigerenzer, Gerd ; Gaissmaier, Wolfgang ; Kurz-Milcke, Elke ; Schwartz, Lisa M. ; Woloshin, Steven</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2007</creationdate><topic>Breast cancer</topic><topic>Cancer screening</topic><topic>Citizenship</topic><topic>Cognitive bias</topic><topic>Conflicts of interest</topic><topic>Democracy</topic><topic>Determinism</topic><topic>Frame analysis</topic><topic>Group decision making</topic><topic>Health care</topic><topic>Health care statistics</topic><topic>Illiteracy</topic><topic>Informed consent</topic><topic>Journalists</topic><topic>Leaflets</topic><topic>Literacy</topic><topic>Mammography</topic><topic>Manipulation</topic><topic>Meaning</topic><topic>Medical journals</topic><topic>Medical schools</topic><topic>Medical screening</topic><topic>Mortality</topic><topic>Natural frequencies</topic><topic>Paternalism</topic><topic>Pharmaceutical industry</topic><topic>Physician patient relationships</topic><topic>Physicians</topic><topic>Politicians</topic><topic>Probabilities</topic><topic>Statistics</topic><topic>World problems</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Gigerenzer, Gerd</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gaissmaier, Wolfgang</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kurz-Milcke, Elke</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schwartz, Lisa M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Woloshin, Steven</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Risk Abstracts</collection><collection>Safety Science and Risk</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><jtitle>Psychological science in the public interest</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Gigerenzer, Gerd</au><au>Gaissmaier, Wolfgang</au><au>Kurz-Milcke, Elke</au><au>Schwartz, Lisa M.</au><au>Woloshin, Steven</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics</atitle><jtitle>Psychological science in the public interest</jtitle><addtitle>Psychol Sci Public Interest</addtitle><date>2007-11-01</date><risdate>2007</risdate><volume>8</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>53</spage><epage>96</epage><pages>53-96</pages><issn>1529-1006</issn><eissn>2160-0031</eissn><eissn>1539-6053</eissn><abstract>Many doctors, patients, journalists, and politicians alike do not understand what health statistics mean or draw wrong conclusions without noticing. Collective statistical illiteracy refers to the widespread inability to understand the meaning of numbers. For instance, many citizens are unaware that higher survival rates with cancer screening do not imply longer life, or that the statement that mammography screening reduces the risk of dying from breast cancer by 25% in fact means that 1 less woman out of 1,000 will die of the disease. We provide evidence that statistical illiteracy (a) is common to patients, journalists, and physicians; (b) is created by nontransparent framing of information that is sometimes an unintentional result of lack of understanding but can also be a result of intentional efforts to manipulate or persuade people; and (c) can have serious consequences for health. The causes of statistical illiteracy should not be attributed to cognitive biases alone, but to the emotional nature of the doctor-patient relationship and conflicts of interest in the healthcare system. The classic doctor-patient relation is based on (the physician's) paternalism and (the patient's) trust in authority, which make statistical literacy seem unnecessary; so does the traditional combination of determinism (physicians who seek causes, not chances) and the illusion of certainty (patients who seek certainty when there is none). We show that information pamphlets, Web sites, leaflets distributed to doctors by the pharma- ceutical industry, and even medical journals often report evidence in nontransparent forms that suggest big benefits of featured interventions and small harms. Without understanding the numbers involved, the public is susceptible to political and commercial manipulation of their anxieties and hopes, which undermines the goals of informed consent and shared decision making. What can be done? We discuss the importance of teaching statistical thinking and transparent representations in primary and secondary education as well as in medical school. Yet this requires familiarizing children early on with the concept of probability and teaching statistical literacy as the art of solving real-world problems rather than applying formulas to toy problems about coins and dice. A major precondition for statistical literacy is transparent risk communication. We recommend using frequency statements instead of single-event probabilities, absolute risks instead of relative risks, mortality rates instead of survival rates, and natural frequencies instead of conditional probabilities. Psychological research on transparent visual and numerical forms of risk communication, as well as training of physicians in their use, is called for. Statistical literacy is a necessary precondition for an educated citizenship in a technological democracy. Understanding risks and asking critical questions can also shape the emotional climate in a society so that hopes and anxieties are no longer as easily manipulated from outside and citizens can develop a better-informed and more relaxed attitude toward their health.</abstract><cop>Los Angeles, CA</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishers</pub><pmid>26161749</pmid><doi>10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x</doi><tpages>44</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1529-1006 |
ispartof | Psychological science in the public interest, 2007-11, Vol.8 (2), p.53-96 |
issn | 1529-1006 2160-0031 1539-6053 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_19618730 |
source | Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Business Source Ultimate; JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection; Sage Journals Online |
subjects | Breast cancer Cancer screening Citizenship Cognitive bias Conflicts of interest Democracy Determinism Frame analysis Group decision making Health care Health care statistics Illiteracy Informed consent Journalists Leaflets Literacy Mammography Manipulation Meaning Medical journals Medical schools Medical screening Mortality Natural frequencies Paternalism Pharmaceutical industry Physician patient relationships Physicians Politicians Probabilities Statistics World problems |
title | Helping Doctors and Patients Make Sense of Health Statistics |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-27T22%3A56%3A49IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Helping%20Doctors%20and%20Patients%20Make%20Sense%20of%20Health%20Statistics&rft.jtitle=Psychological%20science%20in%20the%20public%20interest&rft.au=Gigerenzer,%20Gerd&rft.date=2007-11-01&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=53&rft.epage=96&rft.pages=53-96&rft.issn=1529-1006&rft.eissn=2160-0031&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E40062369%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c473x-14583b0ad30a25f3aac2659ea8a9e3620441e087b26162e36416fd32dc8805523%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1882869934&rft_id=info:pmid/26161749&rft_jstor_id=40062369&rft_sage_id=10.1111_j.1539-6053.2008.00033.x&rfr_iscdi=true |