Loading…
Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment?
Previous studies have found that Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice use a predator’s head orientation to determine risk, taking fewer seeds from a feeder if an avian predator model’s head is facing the feeder while ignoring the head orientation. In addition to head orientation, eyes are a cue of...
Saved in:
Published in: | Animal cognition 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.533-540 |
---|---|
Main Author: | |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3 |
container_end_page | 540 |
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 533 |
container_title | Animal cognition |
container_volume | 24 |
creator | Kyle, Steven C. |
description | Previous studies have found that Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice use a predator’s head orientation to determine risk, taking fewer seeds from a feeder if an avian predator model’s head is facing the feeder while ignoring the head orientation. In addition to head orientation, eyes are a cue of predator risk. In the current study, I examined whether or not chickadees and titmice used the presence of eyes of a predator model to determine when to forage for food. Plastic owl models, with their eyes covered or uncovered, were presented to wild flocks of chickadees and titmice. To test whether or not chickadees and titmice would respond to the presence of eyes, the number of seeds taken and the calling behavior of birds were compared between the two types of predator presentations (eyes covered or uncovered). Chickadees and titmice took fewer seeds when the eyes were uncovered than when they were covered. Chickadees also gave significantly more introductory notes, often used in association with the presence of predators or risk, in their calls when the eyes were visible than when the eyes of the predator model were covered. The results indicate that chickadees and titmice can use the presence of eyes on predators to determine predation risk and possibly use eye gaze to determine where a predator is looking. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s10071-020-01449-1 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2461862947</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2528309042</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kctu1TAQhq0K1Bt9gS6QJTani4DvcVYITrlJlWBB15bjTKjbJD71JIuKl8clvUgsWNgzmvnmn5F-Qk45e8sZq9_h_c8rJljFuFJNxffIIVdSV43S5sVTruwBOUK8ZoxZ1fB9ciClYLq8Q_L7PNGtz2mIk6fhKoYb3wEg3fxIEOIANKxNmDDiGfVTR-eln6GEOI8xAN189JCGtLtakLYxlDSf0QWB7jJ0fk6Zwl0RjBPNEW-oRwTEEab5_SvysvcDwslDPCaXnz_93H6tLr5_-bb9cFEFWeu5ai2ExmgjTW-Y73gwjQ_CG2h110kttOeyltbYTtZWytaWWt-wVoIImutWHpPNqrvL6XYBnN0YMcAw-AnSgk4ow60RjaoL-uYf9DoteSrXOaGFlaxhShRKrFTICTFD73Y5jj7fOc7cvSlutcYVa9xfaxwvQ68fpJd2hO5p5NGLAsgVwNKafkF-3v0f2T_Fe5kT</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2528309042</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment?</title><source>Springer Nature</source><creator>Kyle, Steven C.</creator><creatorcontrib>Kyle, Steven C.</creatorcontrib><description>Previous studies have found that Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice use a predator’s head orientation to determine risk, taking fewer seeds from a feeder if an avian predator model’s head is facing the feeder while ignoring the head orientation. In addition to head orientation, eyes are a cue of predator risk. In the current study, I examined whether or not chickadees and titmice used the presence of eyes of a predator model to determine when to forage for food. Plastic owl models, with their eyes covered or uncovered, were presented to wild flocks of chickadees and titmice. To test whether or not chickadees and titmice would respond to the presence of eyes, the number of seeds taken and the calling behavior of birds were compared between the two types of predator presentations (eyes covered or uncovered). Chickadees and titmice took fewer seeds when the eyes were uncovered than when they were covered. Chickadees also gave significantly more introductory notes, often used in association with the presence of predators or risk, in their calls when the eyes were visible than when the eyes of the predator model were covered. The results indicate that chickadees and titmice can use the presence of eyes on predators to determine predation risk and possibly use eye gaze to determine where a predator is looking.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1435-9448</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1435-9456</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s10071-020-01449-1</identifier><identifier>PMID: 33205320</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Animal behavior ; Animal cognition ; Behavioral Sciences ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Birds ; Calling behavior ; Eye movements ; Life Sciences ; Orientation ; Original Paper ; Predation ; Predators ; Psychology Research ; Risk assessment ; Seeds ; Zoology</subject><ispartof>Animal cognition, 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.533-540</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2020.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,778,782,27907,27908</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33205320$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kyle, Steven C.</creatorcontrib><title>Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment?</title><title>Animal cognition</title><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><description>Previous studies have found that Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice use a predator’s head orientation to determine risk, taking fewer seeds from a feeder if an avian predator model’s head is facing the feeder while ignoring the head orientation. In addition to head orientation, eyes are a cue of predator risk. In the current study, I examined whether or not chickadees and titmice used the presence of eyes of a predator model to determine when to forage for food. Plastic owl models, with their eyes covered or uncovered, were presented to wild flocks of chickadees and titmice. To test whether or not chickadees and titmice would respond to the presence of eyes, the number of seeds taken and the calling behavior of birds were compared between the two types of predator presentations (eyes covered or uncovered). Chickadees and titmice took fewer seeds when the eyes were uncovered than when they were covered. Chickadees also gave significantly more introductory notes, often used in association with the presence of predators or risk, in their calls when the eyes were visible than when the eyes of the predator model were covered. The results indicate that chickadees and titmice can use the presence of eyes on predators to determine predation risk and possibly use eye gaze to determine where a predator is looking.</description><subject>Animal behavior</subject><subject>Animal cognition</subject><subject>Behavioral Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Birds</subject><subject>Calling behavior</subject><subject>Eye movements</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>Orientation</subject><subject>Original Paper</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Psychology Research</subject><subject>Risk assessment</subject><subject>Seeds</subject><subject>Zoology</subject><issn>1435-9448</issn><issn>1435-9456</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2021</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kctu1TAQhq0K1Bt9gS6QJTani4DvcVYITrlJlWBB15bjTKjbJD71JIuKl8clvUgsWNgzmvnmn5F-Qk45e8sZq9_h_c8rJljFuFJNxffIIVdSV43S5sVTruwBOUK8ZoxZ1fB9ciClYLq8Q_L7PNGtz2mIk6fhKoYb3wEg3fxIEOIANKxNmDDiGfVTR-eln6GEOI8xAN189JCGtLtakLYxlDSf0QWB7jJ0fk6Zwl0RjBPNEW-oRwTEEab5_SvysvcDwslDPCaXnz_93H6tLr5_-bb9cFEFWeu5ai2ExmgjTW-Y73gwjQ_CG2h110kttOeyltbYTtZWytaWWt-wVoIImutWHpPNqrvL6XYBnN0YMcAw-AnSgk4ow60RjaoL-uYf9DoteSrXOaGFlaxhShRKrFTICTFD73Y5jj7fOc7cvSlutcYVa9xfaxwvQ68fpJd2hO5p5NGLAsgVwNKafkF-3v0f2T_Fe5kT</recordid><startdate>20210501</startdate><enddate>20210501</enddate><creator>Kyle, Steven C.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AEUYN</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20210501</creationdate><title>Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment?</title><author>Kyle, Steven C.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2021</creationdate><topic>Animal behavior</topic><topic>Animal cognition</topic><topic>Behavioral Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Birds</topic><topic>Calling behavior</topic><topic>Eye movements</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>Orientation</topic><topic>Original Paper</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Psychology Research</topic><topic>Risk assessment</topic><topic>Seeds</topic><topic>Zoology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kyle, Steven C.</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Sustainability</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>PML(ProQuest Medical Library)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>Proquest Research Library</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Animal cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kyle, Steven C.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment?</atitle><jtitle>Animal cognition</jtitle><stitle>Anim Cogn</stitle><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><date>2021-05-01</date><risdate>2021</risdate><volume>24</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>533</spage><epage>540</epage><pages>533-540</pages><issn>1435-9448</issn><eissn>1435-9456</eissn><abstract>Previous studies have found that Carolina chickadees and tufted titmice use a predator’s head orientation to determine risk, taking fewer seeds from a feeder if an avian predator model’s head is facing the feeder while ignoring the head orientation. In addition to head orientation, eyes are a cue of predator risk. In the current study, I examined whether or not chickadees and titmice used the presence of eyes of a predator model to determine when to forage for food. Plastic owl models, with their eyes covered or uncovered, were presented to wild flocks of chickadees and titmice. To test whether or not chickadees and titmice would respond to the presence of eyes, the number of seeds taken and the calling behavior of birds were compared between the two types of predator presentations (eyes covered or uncovered). Chickadees and titmice took fewer seeds when the eyes were uncovered than when they were covered. Chickadees also gave significantly more introductory notes, often used in association with the presence of predators or risk, in their calls when the eyes were visible than when the eyes of the predator model were covered. The results indicate that chickadees and titmice can use the presence of eyes on predators to determine predation risk and possibly use eye gaze to determine where a predator is looking.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>33205320</pmid><doi>10.1007/s10071-020-01449-1</doi><tpages>8</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1435-9448 |
ispartof | Animal cognition, 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.533-540 |
issn | 1435-9448 1435-9456 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2461862947 |
source | Springer Nature |
subjects | Animal behavior Animal cognition Behavioral Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Birds Calling behavior Eye movements Life Sciences Orientation Original Paper Predation Predators Psychology Research Risk assessment Seeds Zoology |
title | Do Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) and tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor) use predator eyes in risk assessment? |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-17T04%3A20%3A13IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Do%20Carolina%20chickadees%20(Poecile%20carolinensis)%20and%20tufted%20titmice%20(Baeolophus%20bicolor)%20use%20predator%20eyes%20in%20risk%20assessment?&rft.jtitle=Animal%20cognition&rft.au=Kyle,%20Steven%20C.&rft.date=2021-05-01&rft.volume=24&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=533&rft.epage=540&rft.pages=533-540&rft.issn=1435-9448&rft.eissn=1435-9456&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s10071-020-01449-1&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2528309042%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-b8ec965636f60ad1c69ac2a6eb5dd3525a1373868d37833b8352f90b3e2c515b3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2528309042&rft_id=info:pmid/33205320&rfr_iscdi=true |