Loading…

Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development

A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Developmental science 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.e13066-n/a
Main Authors: Motamedi, Yasamin, Murgiano, Margherita, Perniss, Pamela, Wonnacott, Elizabeth, Marshall, Chloë, Goldin‐Meadow, Susan, Vigliocco, Gabriella
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
Description
Summary:A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning. What role does onomatopoeia play in children's vocabulary development? We use naturalistic observations of caregiver‐child interactions as well as a word learning experiment with 2–3 year old English‐speaking children to understand how caregivers use onomatopoea in interactions with their children, and how children learn onomatopoeia in both situated and displaced learning contexts.
ISSN:1363-755X
1467-7687
1467-7687
DOI:10.1111/desc.13066