Loading…

Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces

Objective Our study aims to investigate the effects of grating patterns of perceived roughness on surfaces with ultrasonic friction modulation, and also to examine user performance of identifying different numbers of grating patterns. Background In designing grating-based tactile textures, the width...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Human factors 2023-12, Vol.65 (8), p.1718-1739
Main Authors: Chu, Shaowei, Tu, Huawei
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c320t-922bb68a4e8b89ba5991d5192181a67429aa22f527168a37bbbc4818c391e3723
container_end_page 1739
container_issue 8
container_start_page 1718
container_title Human factors
container_volume 65
creator Chu, Shaowei
Tu, Huawei
description Objective Our study aims to investigate the effects of grating patterns of perceived roughness on surfaces with ultrasonic friction modulation, and also to examine user performance of identifying different numbers of grating patterns. Background In designing grating-based tactile textures, the widths of low- and high-friction zones are a crucial factor for generating grating patterns that convey roughness sensation. However, few studies have explored the design space of efficient grating patterns that users can easily distinguish and identify via roughness perception. Method Two experiments were carried out. In the first experiment, we conducted a magnitude estimation of perceived roughness for both low- and high-friction zones, each with widths of 0.13, 0.25, 0.38, 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 3.5, and 5.5 mm. In the second experiment, we required participants to identify 5 pattern groups with 2–6 patterns respectively. Results Perceived roughness fitted a linear trend for low- or high-friction zones with widths of 0.38 mm or lower. Perceived roughness followed an inverted U-shaped curve for low- or high-friction zones with widths greater than 0.5 mm but less than 2.0 mm. The peak points occurred at the widths of 0.38 mm for both low- and high-friction zones. The statistical analysis indicates that both low- and high-friction zones had similar effects on human perception of surface roughness. In addition, participants could memorize and identify up to four tactile patterns with identification accuracy rates higher than 90% and average reaction time less than 2.2 s. Conclusions The relation between perceived roughness and varying widths of grating patterns follows linear or inverted U-shape trends. Participants could efficiently identify 4 or fewer patterns with high accuracy (>90%) and short reaction time (
doi_str_mv 10.1177/00187208211064025
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2621022467</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1177_00187208211064025</sage_id><sourcerecordid>2901414647</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c320t-922bb68a4e8b89ba5991d5192181a67429aa22f527168a37bbbc4818c391e3723</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kUFP3DAQha2qqGxpf0AvlaVeuAQ8Eye2jxUCikQFatlz5DiTJSjrUNtB4t_X6UIrFXGakd43b2b0GPsE4ghAqWMhQCsUGgFELQVWb9gKKqkKDRrestWiFwuwz97HeCeEqE1ZvWP7ZSVKrU21Yo9r31GIyfpu8Buebomf9j25FPnU8xvr0jASPw82LfK1TYmCz5rn1xQcDQ_U8R_TvLn1FCO_eqDA12MKNk5-cPwsDNkgw9-nbh7tn_bnHHrrKH5ge70dI318qgdsfXZ6c_KtuLw6vzj5elm4EkUqDGLb1tpK0q02ra2Mga4Cg_lFWyuJxlrEvkIFmSpV27ZO5v9daYBKheUBO9z53ofp10wxNdshOhpH62maY4M1gkCUtcrol__Qu2kOPl_XoBEgQdZyoWBHuTDFGKhv7sOwteGxAdEsuTQvcskzn5-c53ZL3d-J5yAycLQDot3Qv7WvO_4GbuuUpg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2901414647</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces</title><source>Sage Journals Online</source><creator>Chu, Shaowei ; Tu, Huawei</creator><creatorcontrib>Chu, Shaowei ; Tu, Huawei</creatorcontrib><description>Objective Our study aims to investigate the effects of grating patterns of perceived roughness on surfaces with ultrasonic friction modulation, and also to examine user performance of identifying different numbers of grating patterns. Background In designing grating-based tactile textures, the widths of low- and high-friction zones are a crucial factor for generating grating patterns that convey roughness sensation. However, few studies have explored the design space of efficient grating patterns that users can easily distinguish and identify via roughness perception. Method Two experiments were carried out. In the first experiment, we conducted a magnitude estimation of perceived roughness for both low- and high-friction zones, each with widths of 0.13, 0.25, 0.38, 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 3.5, and 5.5 mm. In the second experiment, we required participants to identify 5 pattern groups with 2–6 patterns respectively. Results Perceived roughness fitted a linear trend for low- or high-friction zones with widths of 0.38 mm or lower. Perceived roughness followed an inverted U-shaped curve for low- or high-friction zones with widths greater than 0.5 mm but less than 2.0 mm. The peak points occurred at the widths of 0.38 mm for both low- and high-friction zones. The statistical analysis indicates that both low- and high-friction zones had similar effects on human perception of surface roughness. In addition, participants could memorize and identify up to four tactile patterns with identification accuracy rates higher than 90% and average reaction time less than 2.2 s. Conclusions The relation between perceived roughness and varying widths of grating patterns follows linear or inverted U-shape trends. Participants could efficiently identify 4 or fewer patterns with high accuracy (&gt;90%) and short reaction time (&lt;2.2 s). Application Our findings can contribute to tactile interface design such as tactile alphabets and target-approaching indicators.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0018-7208</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1547-8181</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1547-8181</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1177/00187208211064025</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35038895</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications</publisher><subject>Friction ; Humans ; Modulation ; Perception ; Perceptions ; Reaction Time ; Sensation ; Statistical analysis ; Surface roughness ; Touch ; Touch Perception ; Ultrasonics</subject><ispartof>Human factors, 2023-12, Vol.65 (8), p.1718-1739</ispartof><rights>2022 Human Factors and Ergonomics Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c320t-922bb68a4e8b89ba5991d5192181a67429aa22f527168a37bbbc4818c391e3723</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-5520-296X</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,79364</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35038895$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Chu, Shaowei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tu, Huawei</creatorcontrib><title>Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces</title><title>Human factors</title><addtitle>Hum Factors</addtitle><description>Objective Our study aims to investigate the effects of grating patterns of perceived roughness on surfaces with ultrasonic friction modulation, and also to examine user performance of identifying different numbers of grating patterns. Background In designing grating-based tactile textures, the widths of low- and high-friction zones are a crucial factor for generating grating patterns that convey roughness sensation. However, few studies have explored the design space of efficient grating patterns that users can easily distinguish and identify via roughness perception. Method Two experiments were carried out. In the first experiment, we conducted a magnitude estimation of perceived roughness for both low- and high-friction zones, each with widths of 0.13, 0.25, 0.38, 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 3.5, and 5.5 mm. In the second experiment, we required participants to identify 5 pattern groups with 2–6 patterns respectively. Results Perceived roughness fitted a linear trend for low- or high-friction zones with widths of 0.38 mm or lower. Perceived roughness followed an inverted U-shaped curve for low- or high-friction zones with widths greater than 0.5 mm but less than 2.0 mm. The peak points occurred at the widths of 0.38 mm for both low- and high-friction zones. The statistical analysis indicates that both low- and high-friction zones had similar effects on human perception of surface roughness. In addition, participants could memorize and identify up to four tactile patterns with identification accuracy rates higher than 90% and average reaction time less than 2.2 s. Conclusions The relation between perceived roughness and varying widths of grating patterns follows linear or inverted U-shape trends. Participants could efficiently identify 4 or fewer patterns with high accuracy (&gt;90%) and short reaction time (&lt;2.2 s). Application Our findings can contribute to tactile interface design such as tactile alphabets and target-approaching indicators.</description><subject>Friction</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Modulation</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Perceptions</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Sensation</subject><subject>Statistical analysis</subject><subject>Surface roughness</subject><subject>Touch</subject><subject>Touch Perception</subject><subject>Ultrasonics</subject><issn>0018-7208</issn><issn>1547-8181</issn><issn>1547-8181</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp1kUFP3DAQha2qqGxpf0AvlaVeuAQ8Eye2jxUCikQFatlz5DiTJSjrUNtB4t_X6UIrFXGakd43b2b0GPsE4ghAqWMhQCsUGgFELQVWb9gKKqkKDRrestWiFwuwz97HeCeEqE1ZvWP7ZSVKrU21Yo9r31GIyfpu8Buebomf9j25FPnU8xvr0jASPw82LfK1TYmCz5rn1xQcDQ_U8R_TvLn1FCO_eqDA12MKNk5-cPwsDNkgw9-nbh7tn_bnHHrrKH5ge70dI318qgdsfXZ6c_KtuLw6vzj5elm4EkUqDGLb1tpK0q02ra2Mga4Cg_lFWyuJxlrEvkIFmSpV27ZO5v9daYBKheUBO9z53ofp10wxNdshOhpH62maY4M1gkCUtcrol__Qu2kOPl_XoBEgQdZyoWBHuTDFGKhv7sOwteGxAdEsuTQvcskzn5-c53ZL3d-J5yAycLQDot3Qv7WvO_4GbuuUpg</recordid><startdate>202312</startdate><enddate>202312</enddate><creator>Chu, Shaowei</creator><creator>Tu, Huawei</creator><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>Human Factors and Ergonomics Society</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QF</scope><scope>7QQ</scope><scope>7SC</scope><scope>7SE</scope><scope>7SP</scope><scope>7SR</scope><scope>7T2</scope><scope>7TA</scope><scope>7TB</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7U5</scope><scope>8BQ</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>F28</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>H8D</scope><scope>H8G</scope><scope>JG9</scope><scope>JQ2</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KR7</scope><scope>L7M</scope><scope>L~C</scope><scope>L~D</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5520-296X</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202312</creationdate><title>Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces</title><author>Chu, Shaowei ; Tu, Huawei</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c320t-922bb68a4e8b89ba5991d5192181a67429aa22f527168a37bbbc4818c391e3723</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Friction</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Modulation</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Perceptions</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Sensation</topic><topic>Statistical analysis</topic><topic>Surface roughness</topic><topic>Touch</topic><topic>Touch Perception</topic><topic>Ultrasonics</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Chu, Shaowei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Tu, Huawei</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Aluminium Industry Abstracts</collection><collection>Ceramic Abstracts</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts</collection><collection>Corrosion Abstracts</collection><collection>Electronics &amp; Communications Abstracts</collection><collection>Engineered Materials Abstracts</collection><collection>Health and Safety Science Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Materials Business File</collection><collection>Mechanical &amp; Transportation Engineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Solid State and Superconductivity Abstracts</collection><collection>METADEX</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ANTE: Abstracts in New Technology &amp; Engineering</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Aerospace Database</collection><collection>Copper Technical Reference Library</collection><collection>Materials Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Computer Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Civil Engineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies Database with Aerospace</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts – Academic</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Professional</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Human factors</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Chu, Shaowei</au><au>Tu, Huawei</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces</atitle><jtitle>Human factors</jtitle><addtitle>Hum Factors</addtitle><date>2023-12</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>65</volume><issue>8</issue><spage>1718</spage><epage>1739</epage><pages>1718-1739</pages><issn>0018-7208</issn><issn>1547-8181</issn><eissn>1547-8181</eissn><abstract>Objective Our study aims to investigate the effects of grating patterns of perceived roughness on surfaces with ultrasonic friction modulation, and also to examine user performance of identifying different numbers of grating patterns. Background In designing grating-based tactile textures, the widths of low- and high-friction zones are a crucial factor for generating grating patterns that convey roughness sensation. However, few studies have explored the design space of efficient grating patterns that users can easily distinguish and identify via roughness perception. Method Two experiments were carried out. In the first experiment, we conducted a magnitude estimation of perceived roughness for both low- and high-friction zones, each with widths of 0.13, 0.25, 0.38, 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 3.5, and 5.5 mm. In the second experiment, we required participants to identify 5 pattern groups with 2–6 patterns respectively. Results Perceived roughness fitted a linear trend for low- or high-friction zones with widths of 0.38 mm or lower. Perceived roughness followed an inverted U-shaped curve for low- or high-friction zones with widths greater than 0.5 mm but less than 2.0 mm. The peak points occurred at the widths of 0.38 mm for both low- and high-friction zones. The statistical analysis indicates that both low- and high-friction zones had similar effects on human perception of surface roughness. In addition, participants could memorize and identify up to four tactile patterns with identification accuracy rates higher than 90% and average reaction time less than 2.2 s. Conclusions The relation between perceived roughness and varying widths of grating patterns follows linear or inverted U-shape trends. Participants could efficiently identify 4 or fewer patterns with high accuracy (&gt;90%) and short reaction time (&lt;2.2 s). Application Our findings can contribute to tactile interface design such as tactile alphabets and target-approaching indicators.</abstract><cop>Los Angeles, CA</cop><pub>SAGE Publications</pub><pmid>35038895</pmid><doi>10.1177/00187208211064025</doi><tpages>22</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5520-296X</orcidid></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0018-7208
ispartof Human factors, 2023-12, Vol.65 (8), p.1718-1739
issn 0018-7208
1547-8181
1547-8181
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2621022467
source Sage Journals Online
subjects Friction
Humans
Modulation
Perception
Perceptions
Reaction Time
Sensation
Statistical analysis
Surface roughness
Touch
Touch Perception
Ultrasonics
title Understanding the Effects of Tactile Grating Patterns on Perceived Roughness Over Ultrasonic Friction Modulation Surfaces
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-03T18%3A09%3A12IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Understanding%20the%20Effects%20of%20Tactile%20Grating%20Patterns%20on%20Perceived%20Roughness%20Over%20Ultrasonic%20Friction%20Modulation%20Surfaces&rft.jtitle=Human%20factors&rft.au=Chu,%20Shaowei&rft.date=2023-12&rft.volume=65&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=1718&rft.epage=1739&rft.pages=1718-1739&rft.issn=0018-7208&rft.eissn=1547-8181&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177/00187208211064025&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2901414647%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c320t-922bb68a4e8b89ba5991d5192181a67429aa22f527168a37bbbc4818c391e3723%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2901414647&rft_id=info:pmid/35038895&rft_sage_id=10.1177_00187208211064025&rfr_iscdi=true