Loading…

Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?

Previous studies have linked levels of perceived chronic stress to older adults' cognitive functioning, but few have focused on the impact of chronic stressor types. Thus, this study aimed to (1) identify patterns of chronic stressor types and (2) examine the effects of these patterns on cognit...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Social science & medicine (1982) 2022-10, Vol.311, p.115297-115297, Article 115297
Main Authors: Wang, Kun, Marbut, Alexander R., Suntai, Zainab, Zheng, Dianhan, Chen, Xiayu
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c224t-d7d5bca663c614d75d634a695bc3bf815540821d486f98c49245bb9c7aefbe1e3
container_end_page 115297
container_issue
container_start_page 115297
container_title Social science & medicine (1982)
container_volume 311
creator Wang, Kun
Marbut, Alexander R.
Suntai, Zainab
Zheng, Dianhan
Chen, Xiayu
description Previous studies have linked levels of perceived chronic stress to older adults' cognitive functioning, but few have focused on the impact of chronic stressor types. Thus, this study aimed to (1) identify patterns of chronic stressor types and (2) examine the effects of these patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories among older adults. Two longitudinal studies were conducted separately to test the study aims and ensure replicability across samples and time points. Both used three timepoints (Study 1: 2006, 2008, and 2010, n = 6974; Study 2: 2012, 2014, and 2016, n = 6604) collected from older Americans in the Health and Retirement Study. Participants did not overlap between the two studies. Latent class analyses were conducted to identify chronic stressor-type patterns. Latent growth curve models were used to test the effects of chronic stressor-type patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories. Three latent classes of stressor types were identified in both studies: egocentric (4.56%; 5.85%), nonegocentric (8.58%; 10.03%), and low stressor (86.86%; 84.12%). In both studies, compared to the low stressor class, the egocentric stressor class had significantly lower initial cognitive scores (B = −0.72, ρ 
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115297
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2710973981</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0277953622006037</els_id><sourcerecordid>2710973981</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c224t-d7d5bca663c614d75d634a695bc3bf815540821d486f98c49245bb9c7aefbe1e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkc9qGzEQh0VJoY7bZ6huzWUd_dld7eYSTEiagiE9tGehlWZdmbXkaGQHv0SfOTIuOaangZlvPpj5EfKVswVnvL3eLDBatH4LbiGYEAvOG9GrD2TGOyWrRtbqgsyYUKrqG9l-IpeIG8YYZ52ckb8_Tc6QAlIfaJwcJGrcfsr4je4gWfAHcNT-STF4SzEnQIyJ5uMOkJpQRnEdfC4UHffBZl-4sKY5mQ3YHJMHvKHLBO_Iimd6MUekg3G3n8nH0UwIX_7VOfn9cP_r7rFaPX3_cbdcVVaIOldOuWawpm2lbXntVONaWZu2L005jB1vmpp1gru6a8e-s3Uv6mYYeqsMjANwkHNydfbuUnzeA2a99WhhmkyAuEctFGe9kn3HC6rOqE0RMcGod8lvTTpqzvQpAb3RbwnoUwL6nEDZXJ43oVxy8JB0gSBYcD6V72gX_X8drwhWl7E</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2710973981</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?</title><source>ScienceDirect Freedom Collection</source><creator>Wang, Kun ; Marbut, Alexander R. ; Suntai, Zainab ; Zheng, Dianhan ; Chen, Xiayu</creator><creatorcontrib>Wang, Kun ; Marbut, Alexander R. ; Suntai, Zainab ; Zheng, Dianhan ; Chen, Xiayu</creatorcontrib><description>Previous studies have linked levels of perceived chronic stress to older adults' cognitive functioning, but few have focused on the impact of chronic stressor types. Thus, this study aimed to (1) identify patterns of chronic stressor types and (2) examine the effects of these patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories among older adults. Two longitudinal studies were conducted separately to test the study aims and ensure replicability across samples and time points. Both used three timepoints (Study 1: 2006, 2008, and 2010, n = 6974; Study 2: 2012, 2014, and 2016, n = 6604) collected from older Americans in the Health and Retirement Study. Participants did not overlap between the two studies. Latent class analyses were conducted to identify chronic stressor-type patterns. Latent growth curve models were used to test the effects of chronic stressor-type patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories. Three latent classes of stressor types were identified in both studies: egocentric (4.56%; 5.85%), nonegocentric (8.58%; 10.03%), and low stressor (86.86%; 84.12%). In both studies, compared to the low stressor class, the egocentric stressor class had significantly lower initial cognitive scores (B = −0.72, ρ &lt; 0.001; B = −0.46, ρ &lt; 0.05), while the nonegocentric stressor class did not have significantly different initial scores, with covariates controlled. Additionally, in Study 1, the nonegocentric stressor class had significantly slower cognitive decline rates than the low stressor class (B = 0.11, ρ &lt; 0.05). Findings suggested that nonegocentric stressors are an important stressor source in late adulthood but are less detrimental to cognitive functioning than egocentric stressors. Health management interventions may reduce older adults' cognitive health disparities caused by self-health and financial stressors. More support, including financial subsidies, caregiver stress management training, or support groups, should be provided to older caregivers, especially those with few resources. •There were three stressor classes: egocentric, nonegocentric, and low stressors.•The egocentric class had lower initial cognition than the low stressor class.•The nonegocentric class had slower cognitive decline than the low stressor class.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0277-9536</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-5347</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115297</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Elsevier Ltd</publisher><subject>Challenge-hindrance perspective ; Cognitive functioning ; Egocentric stressor ; Nonegocentric stressor ; Older adults</subject><ispartof>Social science &amp; medicine (1982), 2022-10, Vol.311, p.115297-115297, Article 115297</ispartof><rights>2022 Elsevier Ltd</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c224t-d7d5bca663c614d75d634a695bc3bf815540821d486f98c49245bb9c7aefbe1e3</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-3747-3947 ; 0000-0002-0331-6479 ; 0000-0002-0606-1950</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27903,27904</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Wang, Kun</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marbut, Alexander R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suntai, Zainab</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zheng, Dianhan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chen, Xiayu</creatorcontrib><title>Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?</title><title>Social science &amp; medicine (1982)</title><description>Previous studies have linked levels of perceived chronic stress to older adults' cognitive functioning, but few have focused on the impact of chronic stressor types. Thus, this study aimed to (1) identify patterns of chronic stressor types and (2) examine the effects of these patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories among older adults. Two longitudinal studies were conducted separately to test the study aims and ensure replicability across samples and time points. Both used three timepoints (Study 1: 2006, 2008, and 2010, n = 6974; Study 2: 2012, 2014, and 2016, n = 6604) collected from older Americans in the Health and Retirement Study. Participants did not overlap between the two studies. Latent class analyses were conducted to identify chronic stressor-type patterns. Latent growth curve models were used to test the effects of chronic stressor-type patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories. Three latent classes of stressor types were identified in both studies: egocentric (4.56%; 5.85%), nonegocentric (8.58%; 10.03%), and low stressor (86.86%; 84.12%). In both studies, compared to the low stressor class, the egocentric stressor class had significantly lower initial cognitive scores (B = −0.72, ρ &lt; 0.001; B = −0.46, ρ &lt; 0.05), while the nonegocentric stressor class did not have significantly different initial scores, with covariates controlled. Additionally, in Study 1, the nonegocentric stressor class had significantly slower cognitive decline rates than the low stressor class (B = 0.11, ρ &lt; 0.05). Findings suggested that nonegocentric stressors are an important stressor source in late adulthood but are less detrimental to cognitive functioning than egocentric stressors. Health management interventions may reduce older adults' cognitive health disparities caused by self-health and financial stressors. More support, including financial subsidies, caregiver stress management training, or support groups, should be provided to older caregivers, especially those with few resources. •There were three stressor classes: egocentric, nonegocentric, and low stressors.•The egocentric class had lower initial cognition than the low stressor class.•The nonegocentric class had slower cognitive decline than the low stressor class.</description><subject>Challenge-hindrance perspective</subject><subject>Cognitive functioning</subject><subject>Egocentric stressor</subject><subject>Nonegocentric stressor</subject><subject>Older adults</subject><issn>0277-9536</issn><issn>1873-5347</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkc9qGzEQh0VJoY7bZ6huzWUd_dld7eYSTEiagiE9tGehlWZdmbXkaGQHv0SfOTIuOaangZlvPpj5EfKVswVnvL3eLDBatH4LbiGYEAvOG9GrD2TGOyWrRtbqgsyYUKrqG9l-IpeIG8YYZ52ckb8_Tc6QAlIfaJwcJGrcfsr4je4gWfAHcNT-STF4SzEnQIyJ5uMOkJpQRnEdfC4UHffBZl-4sKY5mQ3YHJMHvKHLBO_Iimd6MUekg3G3n8nH0UwIX_7VOfn9cP_r7rFaPX3_cbdcVVaIOldOuWawpm2lbXntVONaWZu2L005jB1vmpp1gru6a8e-s3Uv6mYYeqsMjANwkHNydfbuUnzeA2a99WhhmkyAuEctFGe9kn3HC6rOqE0RMcGod8lvTTpqzvQpAb3RbwnoUwL6nEDZXJ43oVxy8JB0gSBYcD6V72gX_X8drwhWl7E</recordid><startdate>202210</startdate><enddate>202210</enddate><creator>Wang, Kun</creator><creator>Marbut, Alexander R.</creator><creator>Suntai, Zainab</creator><creator>Zheng, Dianhan</creator><creator>Chen, Xiayu</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3747-3947</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0331-6479</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0606-1950</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202210</creationdate><title>Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?</title><author>Wang, Kun ; Marbut, Alexander R. ; Suntai, Zainab ; Zheng, Dianhan ; Chen, Xiayu</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c224t-d7d5bca663c614d75d634a695bc3bf815540821d486f98c49245bb9c7aefbe1e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Challenge-hindrance perspective</topic><topic>Cognitive functioning</topic><topic>Egocentric stressor</topic><topic>Nonegocentric stressor</topic><topic>Older adults</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Wang, Kun</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marbut, Alexander R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Suntai, Zainab</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zheng, Dianhan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chen, Xiayu</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Social science &amp; medicine (1982)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Wang, Kun</au><au>Marbut, Alexander R.</au><au>Suntai, Zainab</au><au>Zheng, Dianhan</au><au>Chen, Xiayu</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?</atitle><jtitle>Social science &amp; medicine (1982)</jtitle><date>2022-10</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>311</volume><spage>115297</spage><epage>115297</epage><pages>115297-115297</pages><artnum>115297</artnum><issn>0277-9536</issn><eissn>1873-5347</eissn><abstract>Previous studies have linked levels of perceived chronic stress to older adults' cognitive functioning, but few have focused on the impact of chronic stressor types. Thus, this study aimed to (1) identify patterns of chronic stressor types and (2) examine the effects of these patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories among older adults. Two longitudinal studies were conducted separately to test the study aims and ensure replicability across samples and time points. Both used three timepoints (Study 1: 2006, 2008, and 2010, n = 6974; Study 2: 2012, 2014, and 2016, n = 6604) collected from older Americans in the Health and Retirement Study. Participants did not overlap between the two studies. Latent class analyses were conducted to identify chronic stressor-type patterns. Latent growth curve models were used to test the effects of chronic stressor-type patterns on cognitive functioning trajectories. Three latent classes of stressor types were identified in both studies: egocentric (4.56%; 5.85%), nonegocentric (8.58%; 10.03%), and low stressor (86.86%; 84.12%). In both studies, compared to the low stressor class, the egocentric stressor class had significantly lower initial cognitive scores (B = −0.72, ρ &lt; 0.001; B = −0.46, ρ &lt; 0.05), while the nonegocentric stressor class did not have significantly different initial scores, with covariates controlled. Additionally, in Study 1, the nonegocentric stressor class had significantly slower cognitive decline rates than the low stressor class (B = 0.11, ρ &lt; 0.05). Findings suggested that nonegocentric stressors are an important stressor source in late adulthood but are less detrimental to cognitive functioning than egocentric stressors. Health management interventions may reduce older adults' cognitive health disparities caused by self-health and financial stressors. More support, including financial subsidies, caregiver stress management training, or support groups, should be provided to older caregivers, especially those with few resources. •There were three stressor classes: egocentric, nonegocentric, and low stressors.•The egocentric class had lower initial cognition than the low stressor class.•The nonegocentric class had slower cognitive decline than the low stressor class.</abstract><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><doi>10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115297</doi><tpages>1</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3747-3947</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0331-6479</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0606-1950</orcidid></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0277-9536
ispartof Social science & medicine (1982), 2022-10, Vol.311, p.115297-115297, Article 115297
issn 0277-9536
1873-5347
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2710973981
source ScienceDirect Freedom Collection
subjects Challenge-hindrance perspective
Cognitive functioning
Egocentric stressor
Nonegocentric stressor
Older adults
title Patterns in older adults' perceived chronic stressor types and cognitive functioning trajectories: Are perceived chronic stressors always bad?
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-26T07%3A25%3A32IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Patterns%20in%20older%20adults'%20perceived%20chronic%20stressor%20types%20and%20cognitive%20functioning%20trajectories:%20Are%20perceived%20chronic%20stressors%20always%20bad?&rft.jtitle=Social%20science%20&%20medicine%20(1982)&rft.au=Wang,%20Kun&rft.date=2022-10&rft.volume=311&rft.spage=115297&rft.epage=115297&rft.pages=115297-115297&rft.artnum=115297&rft.issn=0277-9536&rft.eissn=1873-5347&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115297&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2710973981%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c224t-d7d5bca663c614d75d634a695bc3bf815540821d486f98c49245bb9c7aefbe1e3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2710973981&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true