Loading…
Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes
Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch‐up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which ch...
Saved in:
Published in: | Developmental science 2023-07, Vol.26 (4), p.e13342-n/a |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93 |
container_end_page | n/a |
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | e13342 |
container_title | Developmental science |
container_volume | 26 |
creator | Perry, Lynn K. Kucker, Sarah C. Horst, Jessica S. Samuelson, Larissa K. |
description | Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch‐up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which children will belong to which group. We compare the toddler vocabulary composition of late talkers with different long‐term outcomes. The literature suggests most children with typical development (TD) have vocabularies dominated by names for categories organized by similarity in shape (e.g., cup), which supports a bias to attend to shape when generalizing names of novel nouns—a bias associated with accelerated vocabulary development. Previous work has shown that as a group, late talkers tend to say fewer names for categories organized by shape and are less likely to show a “shape bias” than TD children. Here, in a retrospective analysis of 850 children, we compared the vocabulary composition of groups of toddlers who were late bloomers or persisting late talkers. At Time 1 (13‐27 months), the persisting late talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns than both TD children and late bloomers who “caught up” to typically sized vocabularies months later (18‐38‐months). Additionally, children who received a DLD diagnosis between 4 and 7 years said a significantly smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns in year two than TD children and children with other diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia). These findings bring new insight into sources of heterogeneity amongst late talkers and offer a new metric for assessing risk.
Research Highlights
Toddler vocabulary composition, including the proportion of names for categories organized by shape, like spoon, was used to retrospectively compare outcomes of late talking children
Persisting Late Talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to Late Bloomers (late talkers who later caught up to have typically‐sized vocabularies)
Children with later DLD diagnoses said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to children without a DLD diagnosis
The data illustrate the cascading effects of vocabulary composition on subsequent language development and suggest vocabulary composition may be one important marker of persisting delays |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/desc.13342 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2735166402</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1382148</ericid><sourcerecordid>2828276537</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kU1rFTEUhoNY-mU37pWAmyJMzeckd1XK7VVbLrhQwV3IJGeuKTOT22TG0o34E_yN_hJTp63gwmSRkPc57znkReg5JSe0rDcesjuhnAv2BO1TUatK1Vo9LXde80pJ-WUPHeR8RQgRnNBdtFfepWBc7qPvazsCbroYe0g4JtzZYTPZDWAfckwe0ik-D20LCQYHGYcBj9H7rsDforPN1Nl0i13stzGHMcQB25yjC8XV45swfsVdHDa_fvwcIfV_zeM0lhrIz9BOa7sMR_fnIfr8dvVp-b5af3h3sTxbV05QySrd1Iw4ulB-4YkE3YAjoraaSc4UZc45balfKCob5UFbbRvuW269ANUIu-CH6Hj23aZ4PUEeTR-yg64MBHHKhikuaV0Lwgr66h_0Kk5pKNMZpstWteSqUK9nyqWYc4LWbFPoy18YSsxdKuYuFfMnlQK_vLecmh78I_oQQwFezACk4B7l1SXlmlGhi05n_SZ0cPufVuZ89XE5N_0NbX6j_w</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2828276537</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes</title><source>Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection</source><source>ERIC</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Perry, Lynn K. ; Kucker, Sarah C. ; Horst, Jessica S. ; Samuelson, Larissa K.</creator><creatorcontrib>Perry, Lynn K. ; Kucker, Sarah C. ; Horst, Jessica S. ; Samuelson, Larissa K.</creatorcontrib><description>Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch‐up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which children will belong to which group. We compare the toddler vocabulary composition of late talkers with different long‐term outcomes. The literature suggests most children with typical development (TD) have vocabularies dominated by names for categories organized by similarity in shape (e.g., cup), which supports a bias to attend to shape when generalizing names of novel nouns—a bias associated with accelerated vocabulary development. Previous work has shown that as a group, late talkers tend to say fewer names for categories organized by shape and are less likely to show a “shape bias” than TD children. Here, in a retrospective analysis of 850 children, we compared the vocabulary composition of groups of toddlers who were late bloomers or persisting late talkers. At Time 1 (13‐27 months), the persisting late talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns than both TD children and late bloomers who “caught up” to typically sized vocabularies months later (18‐38‐months). Additionally, children who received a DLD diagnosis between 4 and 7 years said a significantly smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns in year two than TD children and children with other diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia). These findings bring new insight into sources of heterogeneity amongst late talkers and offer a new metric for assessing risk.
Research Highlights
Toddler vocabulary composition, including the proportion of names for categories organized by shape, like spoon, was used to retrospectively compare outcomes of late talking children
Persisting Late Talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to Late Bloomers (late talkers who later caught up to have typically‐sized vocabularies)
Children with later DLD diagnoses said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to children without a DLD diagnosis
The data illustrate the cascading effects of vocabulary composition on subsequent language development and suggest vocabulary composition may be one important marker of persisting delays</description><identifier>ISSN: 1363-755X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-7687</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/desc.13342</identifier><identifier>PMID: 36354235</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Wiley</publisher><subject>Bias ; Child Development ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Clinical Diagnosis ; Comparative Analysis ; Delayed language acquisition ; Delayed Speech ; Developmental disabilities ; developmental language disorder ; Diagnosis ; Dyslexia ; Generalization ; Humans ; Language ; Language Acquisition ; Language Development ; Language Development Disorders - diagnosis ; Language disorders ; Language Impairments ; Language Tests ; Language Usage ; late talker ; Literature ; Naming ; Nouns ; Physical Characteristics ; Retrospective Studies ; Risk Assessment ; shape bias ; Toddlers ; Vocabulary ; Vocabulary Development</subject><ispartof>Developmental science, 2023-07, Vol.26 (4), p.e13342-n/a</ispartof><rights>2022 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.</rights><rights>2023 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-3200-5239 ; 0000-0003-2210-3599 ; 0000-0002-9141-3286 ; 0000-0001-6976-3741</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,31269</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1382148$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36354235$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Perry, Lynn K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kucker, Sarah C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horst, Jessica S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Samuelson, Larissa K.</creatorcontrib><title>Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes</title><title>Developmental science</title><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><description>Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch‐up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which children will belong to which group. We compare the toddler vocabulary composition of late talkers with different long‐term outcomes. The literature suggests most children with typical development (TD) have vocabularies dominated by names for categories organized by similarity in shape (e.g., cup), which supports a bias to attend to shape when generalizing names of novel nouns—a bias associated with accelerated vocabulary development. Previous work has shown that as a group, late talkers tend to say fewer names for categories organized by shape and are less likely to show a “shape bias” than TD children. Here, in a retrospective analysis of 850 children, we compared the vocabulary composition of groups of toddlers who were late bloomers or persisting late talkers. At Time 1 (13‐27 months), the persisting late talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns than both TD children and late bloomers who “caught up” to typically sized vocabularies months later (18‐38‐months). Additionally, children who received a DLD diagnosis between 4 and 7 years said a significantly smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns in year two than TD children and children with other diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia). These findings bring new insight into sources of heterogeneity amongst late talkers and offer a new metric for assessing risk.
Research Highlights
Toddler vocabulary composition, including the proportion of names for categories organized by shape, like spoon, was used to retrospectively compare outcomes of late talking children
Persisting Late Talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to Late Bloomers (late talkers who later caught up to have typically‐sized vocabularies)
Children with later DLD diagnoses said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to children without a DLD diagnosis
The data illustrate the cascading effects of vocabulary composition on subsequent language development and suggest vocabulary composition may be one important marker of persisting delays</description><subject>Bias</subject><subject>Child Development</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Clinical Diagnosis</subject><subject>Comparative Analysis</subject><subject>Delayed language acquisition</subject><subject>Delayed Speech</subject><subject>Developmental disabilities</subject><subject>developmental language disorder</subject><subject>Diagnosis</subject><subject>Dyslexia</subject><subject>Generalization</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Acquisition</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Language Development Disorders - diagnosis</subject><subject>Language disorders</subject><subject>Language Impairments</subject><subject>Language Tests</subject><subject>Language Usage</subject><subject>late talker</subject><subject>Literature</subject><subject>Naming</subject><subject>Nouns</subject><subject>Physical Characteristics</subject><subject>Retrospective Studies</subject><subject>Risk Assessment</subject><subject>shape bias</subject><subject>Toddlers</subject><subject>Vocabulary</subject><subject>Vocabulary Development</subject><issn>1363-755X</issn><issn>1467-7687</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kU1rFTEUhoNY-mU37pWAmyJMzeckd1XK7VVbLrhQwV3IJGeuKTOT22TG0o34E_yN_hJTp63gwmSRkPc57znkReg5JSe0rDcesjuhnAv2BO1TUatK1Vo9LXde80pJ-WUPHeR8RQgRnNBdtFfepWBc7qPvazsCbroYe0g4JtzZYTPZDWAfckwe0ik-D20LCQYHGYcBj9H7rsDforPN1Nl0i13stzGHMcQB25yjC8XV45swfsVdHDa_fvwcIfV_zeM0lhrIz9BOa7sMR_fnIfr8dvVp-b5af3h3sTxbV05QySrd1Iw4ulB-4YkE3YAjoraaSc4UZc45balfKCob5UFbbRvuW269ANUIu-CH6Hj23aZ4PUEeTR-yg64MBHHKhikuaV0Lwgr66h_0Kk5pKNMZpstWteSqUK9nyqWYc4LWbFPoy18YSsxdKuYuFfMnlQK_vLecmh78I_oQQwFezACk4B7l1SXlmlGhi05n_SZ0cPufVuZ89XE5N_0NbX6j_w</recordid><startdate>202307</startdate><enddate>202307</enddate><creator>Perry, Lynn K.</creator><creator>Kucker, Sarah C.</creator><creator>Horst, Jessica S.</creator><creator>Samuelson, Larissa K.</creator><general>Wiley</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3200-5239</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2210-3599</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9141-3286</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6976-3741</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202307</creationdate><title>Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes</title><author>Perry, Lynn K. ; Kucker, Sarah C. ; Horst, Jessica S. ; Samuelson, Larissa K.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Bias</topic><topic>Child Development</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Clinical Diagnosis</topic><topic>Comparative Analysis</topic><topic>Delayed language acquisition</topic><topic>Delayed Speech</topic><topic>Developmental disabilities</topic><topic>developmental language disorder</topic><topic>Diagnosis</topic><topic>Dyslexia</topic><topic>Generalization</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Acquisition</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Language Development Disorders - diagnosis</topic><topic>Language disorders</topic><topic>Language Impairments</topic><topic>Language Tests</topic><topic>Language Usage</topic><topic>late talker</topic><topic>Literature</topic><topic>Naming</topic><topic>Nouns</topic><topic>Physical Characteristics</topic><topic>Retrospective Studies</topic><topic>Risk Assessment</topic><topic>shape bias</topic><topic>Toddlers</topic><topic>Vocabulary</topic><topic>Vocabulary Development</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Perry, Lynn K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kucker, Sarah C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Horst, Jessica S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Samuelson, Larissa K.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Perry, Lynn K.</au><au>Kucker, Sarah C.</au><au>Horst, Jessica S.</au><au>Samuelson, Larissa K.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1382148</ericid><atitle>Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes</atitle><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><date>2023-07</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>e13342</spage><epage>n/a</epage><pages>e13342-n/a</pages><issn>1363-755X</issn><eissn>1467-7687</eissn><abstract>Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch‐up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which children will belong to which group. We compare the toddler vocabulary composition of late talkers with different long‐term outcomes. The literature suggests most children with typical development (TD) have vocabularies dominated by names for categories organized by similarity in shape (e.g., cup), which supports a bias to attend to shape when generalizing names of novel nouns—a bias associated with accelerated vocabulary development. Previous work has shown that as a group, late talkers tend to say fewer names for categories organized by shape and are less likely to show a “shape bias” than TD children. Here, in a retrospective analysis of 850 children, we compared the vocabulary composition of groups of toddlers who were late bloomers or persisting late talkers. At Time 1 (13‐27 months), the persisting late talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns than both TD children and late bloomers who “caught up” to typically sized vocabularies months later (18‐38‐months). Additionally, children who received a DLD diagnosis between 4 and 7 years said a significantly smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns in year two than TD children and children with other diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia). These findings bring new insight into sources of heterogeneity amongst late talkers and offer a new metric for assessing risk.
Research Highlights
Toddler vocabulary composition, including the proportion of names for categories organized by shape, like spoon, was used to retrospectively compare outcomes of late talking children
Persisting Late Talkers said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to Late Bloomers (late talkers who later caught up to have typically‐sized vocabularies)
Children with later DLD diagnoses said a smaller proportion of shape‐based nouns during toddlerhood relative to children without a DLD diagnosis
The data illustrate the cascading effects of vocabulary composition on subsequent language development and suggest vocabulary composition may be one important marker of persisting delays</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Wiley</pub><pmid>36354235</pmid><doi>10.1111/desc.13342</doi><tpages>16</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3200-5239</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2210-3599</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9141-3286</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6976-3741</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1363-755X |
ispartof | Developmental science, 2023-07, Vol.26 (4), p.e13342-n/a |
issn | 1363-755X 1467-7687 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2735166402 |
source | Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection; ERIC; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
subjects | Bias Child Development Child, Preschool Children Clinical Diagnosis Comparative Analysis Delayed language acquisition Delayed Speech Developmental disabilities developmental language disorder Diagnosis Dyslexia Generalization Humans Language Language Acquisition Language Development Language Development Disorders - diagnosis Language disorders Language Impairments Language Tests Language Usage late talker Literature Naming Nouns Physical Characteristics Retrospective Studies Risk Assessment shape bias Toddlers Vocabulary Vocabulary Development |
title | Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-26T21%3A00%3A35IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Late%20bloomer%20or%20language%20disorder?%20Differences%20in%20toddler%20vocabulary%20composition%20associated%20with%20long%E2%80%90term%20language%20outcomes&rft.jtitle=Developmental%20science&rft.au=Perry,%20Lynn%20K.&rft.date=2023-07&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e13342&rft.epage=n/a&rft.pages=e13342-n/a&rft.issn=1363-755X&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/desc.13342&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2828276537%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4152-8b620c197d9d05e8bec046a82532712ccc8a1d9715b7de8a8ab3df3ad4e7b4a93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2828276537&rft_id=info:pmid/36354235&rft_ericid=EJ1382148&rfr_iscdi=true |