Loading…

Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design

This paper presents the results of a qualitative study of problem solving in the domain of instructional design. The study was conducted in two phases: Phase 1 consisted of a "think-aloud" design task with nine instructional designers, five experts and four novices. The results of Phase 2...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Instructional science 1995-11, Vol.23 (5/6), p.321-349
Main Authors: PEREZ, RAY S., JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING, EMERY, CATHY D.
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33
container_end_page 349
container_issue 5/6
container_start_page 321
container_title Instructional science
container_volume 23
creator PEREZ, RAY S.
JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING
EMERY, CATHY D.
description This paper presents the results of a qualitative study of problem solving in the domain of instructional design. The study was conducted in two phases: Phase 1 consisted of a "think-aloud" design task with nine instructional designers, five experts and four novices. The results of Phase 2 are reported in this paper. The design task consisted of presenting the subjects with a description of a fictitious piece of equipment (a simulation of a diesel Cummins engine) and audio and video taping the participants while they produced a lesson for training subjects to trouble shoot a diesel engine simulator. Protocols were then coded using a coding scheme adapted from the work of Greeno et al. (1990). This coding scheme considers three aspects of the problem solving process involved in the development of training: subproblems, types of knowledge used, and problem solving operators. Quantitative and qualitative analysis of the "think aloud" protocols yielded a picture of common problem solving strategies and general features used by "experts" and "novices" during the design process. More importantly, the data showed that experts and novices use divergent design models. These design models differ with respect to the problem solving strategies used by the experts and novices. Experts are more apt to use more design principles and rely on a variety of knowledge sources than novices. Experts spend more time in front-end-analysis or planning and trying to understand the domain than novices, while novices immediately begin to consider in detail numerous design strategies. Expert design models can be characterized by breadth first with considerable elaboration between interconnections while novices' design models are depth first processing with few interconnecting linkages. Moreover, the expert design model is one of integrating, reiterating and cycling through the design process. The expert design process is not a deterministic linear activity, but rather an iterative activity that requires creativity as well as logic.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/BF00896877
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_38802480</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ518513</ericid><jstor_id>23370936</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>23370936</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpF0E1LAzEQBuAgCtbqxbPCnjwIq5PvrJ5qabVS8KLnJZtNSsp2U5Nt0X9vy5Z6msP7MMO8CF1jeMAA8vFlCqAKoaQ8QQPMJc1xwckpGgAQyBmR8hxdpLQEAMwUDNDzrE1d3JjOh1Y3WW2TX7SZ_Vnb2Plkn7JRZsKi9Z3f2mwVattkwR3YJTpzukn26jCH6Gs6-Ry_5fOP19l4NM8NZaLLVWGMM8AJWFZrZahgqmKYCYpZxSohOMEF05RJwphztWOYY0cFVEpo5Sgdort-7zqG741NXbnyydim0a0Nm1RSpYDsvtnB-x6aGFKK1pXr6Fc6_pYYyn0_5X8_O3zTYxu9OcLJO8eK4_3R2z5epi7EY04olVBQQf8AWxpo5Q</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>38802480</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design</title><source>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</source><source>Springer Online Journals Archive Complete</source><source>JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection</source><source>ERIC</source><creator>PEREZ, RAY S. ; JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING ; EMERY, CATHY D.</creator><creatorcontrib>PEREZ, RAY S. ; JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING ; EMERY, CATHY D.</creatorcontrib><description>This paper presents the results of a qualitative study of problem solving in the domain of instructional design. The study was conducted in two phases: Phase 1 consisted of a "think-aloud" design task with nine instructional designers, five experts and four novices. The results of Phase 2 are reported in this paper. The design task consisted of presenting the subjects with a description of a fictitious piece of equipment (a simulation of a diesel Cummins engine) and audio and video taping the participants while they produced a lesson for training subjects to trouble shoot a diesel engine simulator. Protocols were then coded using a coding scheme adapted from the work of Greeno et al. (1990). This coding scheme considers three aspects of the problem solving process involved in the development of training: subproblems, types of knowledge used, and problem solving operators. Quantitative and qualitative analysis of the "think aloud" protocols yielded a picture of common problem solving strategies and general features used by "experts" and "novices" during the design process. More importantly, the data showed that experts and novices use divergent design models. These design models differ with respect to the problem solving strategies used by the experts and novices. Experts are more apt to use more design principles and rely on a variety of knowledge sources than novices. Experts spend more time in front-end-analysis or planning and trying to understand the domain than novices, while novices immediately begin to consider in detail numerous design strategies. Expert design models can be characterized by breadth first with considerable elaboration between interconnections while novices' design models are depth first processing with few interconnecting linkages. Moreover, the expert design model is one of integrating, reiterating and cycling through the design process. The expert design process is not a deterministic linear activity, but rather an iterative activity that requires creativity as well as logic.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0020-4277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1573-1952</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/BF00896877</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Kluwer Academic Publishers</publisher><subject>Cognition ; Cognitive models ; Cognitive Processes ; Comparative Analysis ; Design ; Design analysis ; Design evaluation ; Diesel engines ; Experience ; Expert Novice Problem Solving ; Experts ; Heuristics ; Instructional Design ; Instructional Design Professionals ; Instructional development ; Interpretive Skills ; Learning ; Novices ; Problem Solving ; Protocol Analysis ; Qualitative Research ; Thinking Skills ; Training</subject><ispartof>Instructional science, 1995-11, Vol.23 (5/6), p.321-349</ispartof><rights>1996 Kluwer Academic Publishers</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/23370936$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/23370936$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,33201,58213,58446</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ518513$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>PEREZ, RAY S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>EMERY, CATHY D.</creatorcontrib><title>Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design</title><title>Instructional science</title><description>This paper presents the results of a qualitative study of problem solving in the domain of instructional design. The study was conducted in two phases: Phase 1 consisted of a "think-aloud" design task with nine instructional designers, five experts and four novices. The results of Phase 2 are reported in this paper. The design task consisted of presenting the subjects with a description of a fictitious piece of equipment (a simulation of a diesel Cummins engine) and audio and video taping the participants while they produced a lesson for training subjects to trouble shoot a diesel engine simulator. Protocols were then coded using a coding scheme adapted from the work of Greeno et al. (1990). This coding scheme considers three aspects of the problem solving process involved in the development of training: subproblems, types of knowledge used, and problem solving operators. Quantitative and qualitative analysis of the "think aloud" protocols yielded a picture of common problem solving strategies and general features used by "experts" and "novices" during the design process. More importantly, the data showed that experts and novices use divergent design models. These design models differ with respect to the problem solving strategies used by the experts and novices. Experts are more apt to use more design principles and rely on a variety of knowledge sources than novices. Experts spend more time in front-end-analysis or planning and trying to understand the domain than novices, while novices immediately begin to consider in detail numerous design strategies. Expert design models can be characterized by breadth first with considerable elaboration between interconnections while novices' design models are depth first processing with few interconnecting linkages. Moreover, the expert design model is one of integrating, reiterating and cycling through the design process. The expert design process is not a deterministic linear activity, but rather an iterative activity that requires creativity as well as logic.</description><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive models</subject><subject>Cognitive Processes</subject><subject>Comparative Analysis</subject><subject>Design</subject><subject>Design analysis</subject><subject>Design evaluation</subject><subject>Diesel engines</subject><subject>Experience</subject><subject>Expert Novice Problem Solving</subject><subject>Experts</subject><subject>Heuristics</subject><subject>Instructional Design</subject><subject>Instructional Design Professionals</subject><subject>Instructional development</subject><subject>Interpretive Skills</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Novices</subject><subject>Problem Solving</subject><subject>Protocol Analysis</subject><subject>Qualitative Research</subject><subject>Thinking Skills</subject><subject>Training</subject><issn>0020-4277</issn><issn>1573-1952</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1995</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>8BJ</sourceid><recordid>eNpF0E1LAzEQBuAgCtbqxbPCnjwIq5PvrJ5qabVS8KLnJZtNSsp2U5Nt0X9vy5Z6msP7MMO8CF1jeMAA8vFlCqAKoaQ8QQPMJc1xwckpGgAQyBmR8hxdpLQEAMwUDNDzrE1d3JjOh1Y3WW2TX7SZ_Vnb2Plkn7JRZsKi9Z3f2mwVattkwR3YJTpzukn26jCH6Gs6-Ry_5fOP19l4NM8NZaLLVWGMM8AJWFZrZahgqmKYCYpZxSohOMEF05RJwphztWOYY0cFVEpo5Sgdort-7zqG741NXbnyydim0a0Nm1RSpYDsvtnB-x6aGFKK1pXr6Fc6_pYYyn0_5X8_O3zTYxu9OcLJO8eK4_3R2z5epi7EY04olVBQQf8AWxpo5Q</recordid><startdate>19951101</startdate><enddate>19951101</enddate><creator>PEREZ, RAY S.</creator><creator>JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING</creator><creator>EMERY, CATHY D.</creator><general>Kluwer Academic Publishers</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19951101</creationdate><title>Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design</title><author>PEREZ, RAY S. ; JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING ; EMERY, CATHY D.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1995</creationdate><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive models</topic><topic>Cognitive Processes</topic><topic>Comparative Analysis</topic><topic>Design</topic><topic>Design analysis</topic><topic>Design evaluation</topic><topic>Diesel engines</topic><topic>Experience</topic><topic>Expert Novice Problem Solving</topic><topic>Experts</topic><topic>Heuristics</topic><topic>Instructional Design</topic><topic>Instructional Design Professionals</topic><topic>Instructional development</topic><topic>Interpretive Skills</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Novices</topic><topic>Problem Solving</topic><topic>Protocol Analysis</topic><topic>Qualitative Research</topic><topic>Thinking Skills</topic><topic>Training</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>PEREZ, RAY S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>EMERY, CATHY D.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><jtitle>Instructional science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>PEREZ, RAY S.</au><au>JOHNSON, JULIE FLEMING</au><au>EMERY, CATHY D.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ518513</ericid><atitle>Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design</atitle><jtitle>Instructional science</jtitle><date>1995-11-01</date><risdate>1995</risdate><volume>23</volume><issue>5/6</issue><spage>321</spage><epage>349</epage><pages>321-349</pages><issn>0020-4277</issn><eissn>1573-1952</eissn><abstract>This paper presents the results of a qualitative study of problem solving in the domain of instructional design. The study was conducted in two phases: Phase 1 consisted of a "think-aloud" design task with nine instructional designers, five experts and four novices. The results of Phase 2 are reported in this paper. The design task consisted of presenting the subjects with a description of a fictitious piece of equipment (a simulation of a diesel Cummins engine) and audio and video taping the participants while they produced a lesson for training subjects to trouble shoot a diesel engine simulator. Protocols were then coded using a coding scheme adapted from the work of Greeno et al. (1990). This coding scheme considers three aspects of the problem solving process involved in the development of training: subproblems, types of knowledge used, and problem solving operators. Quantitative and qualitative analysis of the "think aloud" protocols yielded a picture of common problem solving strategies and general features used by "experts" and "novices" during the design process. More importantly, the data showed that experts and novices use divergent design models. These design models differ with respect to the problem solving strategies used by the experts and novices. Experts are more apt to use more design principles and rely on a variety of knowledge sources than novices. Experts spend more time in front-end-analysis or planning and trying to understand the domain than novices, while novices immediately begin to consider in detail numerous design strategies. Expert design models can be characterized by breadth first with considerable elaboration between interconnections while novices' design models are depth first processing with few interconnecting linkages. Moreover, the expert design model is one of integrating, reiterating and cycling through the design process. The expert design process is not a deterministic linear activity, but rather an iterative activity that requires creativity as well as logic.</abstract><pub>Kluwer Academic Publishers</pub><doi>10.1007/BF00896877</doi><tpages>29</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0020-4277
ispartof Instructional science, 1995-11, Vol.23 (5/6), p.321-349
issn 0020-4277
1573-1952
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_38802480
source International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS); Springer Online Journals Archive Complete; JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection; ERIC
subjects Cognition
Cognitive models
Cognitive Processes
Comparative Analysis
Design
Design analysis
Design evaluation
Diesel engines
Experience
Expert Novice Problem Solving
Experts
Heuristics
Instructional Design
Instructional Design Professionals
Instructional development
Interpretive Skills
Learning
Novices
Problem Solving
Protocol Analysis
Qualitative Research
Thinking Skills
Training
title Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-13T21%3A11%3A36IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Instructional%20design%20expertise:%20A%20cognitive%20model%20of%20design&rft.jtitle=Instructional%20science&rft.au=PEREZ,%20RAY%20S.&rft.date=1995-11-01&rft.volume=23&rft.issue=5/6&rft.spage=321&rft.epage=349&rft.pages=321-349&rft.issn=0020-4277&rft.eissn=1573-1952&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/BF00896877&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E23370936%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-89ccfc0520e4da8c3648b4146314b4b6652194a347244ffdf4151f360b86a8f33%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=38802480&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_ericid=EJ518513&rft_jstor_id=23370936&rfr_iscdi=true