Loading…

Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis

Quechua, which is spoken from the southern parts of Colombia to the northern parts of Chile and Argentina, exhibits a rich verbal morphology. The various dialects of Quechua can be ranked in terms of developmental stages, according to the way in which the inflectional system is organized. Some Peruv...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Lingua 1998-08, Vol.105 (3), p.113-148
Main Authors: Lakämper, Renate, Wunderlich, Dieter
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413
container_end_page 148
container_issue 3
container_start_page 113
container_title Lingua
container_volume 105
creator Lakämper, Renate
Wunderlich, Dieter
description Quechua, which is spoken from the southern parts of Colombia to the northern parts of Chile and Argentina, exhibits a rich verbal morphology. The various dialects of Quechua can be ranked in terms of developmental stages, according to the way in which the inflectional system is organized. Some Peruvian dialects (such as those of Ancash and Ayacucho, belonging to different dialect groups) exhibit remarkable asymmetries in the marking of objects : 1st person objects are marked transparently with separate object and subject affixes, whereas the marking of 2nd person objects is somewhat opaque. We account for this asymmetry by the assumption that the person hierarchy in Quechua is 1 ⪢ 2 ⪢ 3 and that the marking of objects is constrained by the Object-Subject-Constraint (OSC). ‘The object may be marked separately from the subject only if it refers to a person that is higher on the hierarchy of person than the person to which the subject refers’. We will then show that the dialect of Cuzco (Peru) has lost any independent morpheme for 2nd person objects, hence, OSC has become irrelevant. It is this stage that allows reanalysis: the most recent dialects such as those of Potosí (Bolivia) and Santiago del Estero (Argentina) have reinvented a 2nd person object morpheme and have given up OSC; in these dialects a symmetric system of object marking has evolved. We will discuss our findings in terms of two possible types of inflectional systems: one type has a small set of morphemes but additional constraints that restrict the actual combinations of morphemes — and so induce asymmetries, whereas the other type has a greater and more finely differentiated set of morphemes but does not need additional constraints — and so allows more transparency and symmetry in the system. We will also show how the development of the Quechua person system is intermingled with that of the number system. Only if the plural suffix follows the person affixes, as happens in the Quechua II dialects, can plural allomorphs arise that are sensitive to person or that fuse plural and person into one single morpheme, facts that have considerable consequences for the whole system.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00010-2
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_58378343</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0024384198000102</els_id><sourcerecordid>58378343</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkMtKxEAQRRtRcHx8gpCFiC6i1Y8kPSuR8QmCirpuOt0Vbc10tCsjuPMj_EK_xIwjbl3V5ty6VYexLQ77HHh5cAsgVC614rtjvQcAHHKxxEZcVyIvuSyW2egPWWVrRE9zSI3HI3Z8jYm6mE1teg7xIQsxu5mhe5zZ7OvjMzvKXBepTzbEPq8toc-mIYapbQP1mY22fadAG2ylsS3h5u9cZ_enJ3eT8_zy6uxicnSZO1lWfY5OSnAohW9E06iqLqQUSvHaC7BeVb5RWhfSYal9XWLtagBbNZUH0AIUl-tsZ7H3JXWvM6TeTAM5bFsbsZuRKbSstFRyAIsF6FJHlLAxL2k4Or0bDmbuzPw4M3MhZqzNjzMjhtz2b4ElZ9sm2egC_YUFcC0EDNjhAsPh2beAyZALGB36kND1xnfhn6JvpdKAqw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>58378343</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis</title><source>ScienceDirect Freedom Collection</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Lakämper, Renate ; Wunderlich, Dieter</creator><creatorcontrib>Lakämper, Renate ; Wunderlich, Dieter</creatorcontrib><description>Quechua, which is spoken from the southern parts of Colombia to the northern parts of Chile and Argentina, exhibits a rich verbal morphology. The various dialects of Quechua can be ranked in terms of developmental stages, according to the way in which the inflectional system is organized. Some Peruvian dialects (such as those of Ancash and Ayacucho, belonging to different dialect groups) exhibit remarkable asymmetries in the marking of objects : 1st person objects are marked transparently with separate object and subject affixes, whereas the marking of 2nd person objects is somewhat opaque. We account for this asymmetry by the assumption that the person hierarchy in Quechua is 1 ⪢ 2 ⪢ 3 and that the marking of objects is constrained by the Object-Subject-Constraint (OSC). ‘The object may be marked separately from the subject only if it refers to a person that is higher on the hierarchy of person than the person to which the subject refers’. We will then show that the dialect of Cuzco (Peru) has lost any independent morpheme for 2nd person objects, hence, OSC has become irrelevant. It is this stage that allows reanalysis: the most recent dialects such as those of Potosí (Bolivia) and Santiago del Estero (Argentina) have reinvented a 2nd person object morpheme and have given up OSC; in these dialects a symmetric system of object marking has evolved. We will discuss our findings in terms of two possible types of inflectional systems: one type has a small set of morphemes but additional constraints that restrict the actual combinations of morphemes — and so induce asymmetries, whereas the other type has a greater and more finely differentiated set of morphemes but does not need additional constraints — and so allows more transparency and symmetry in the system. We will also show how the development of the Quechua person system is intermingled with that of the number system. Only if the plural suffix follows the person affixes, as happens in the Quechua II dialects, can plural allomorphs arise that are sensitive to person or that fuse plural and person into one single morpheme, facts that have considerable consequences for the whole system.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0024-3841</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1872-6135</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00010-2</identifier><identifier>CODEN: LINGAO</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Constraint-based morphology ; Descriptive studies and applied theories ; Linguistics ; Morphology ; Object agreement ; Quechua</subject><ispartof>Lingua, 1998-08, Vol.105 (3), p.113-148</ispartof><rights>1998</rights><rights>1999 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,31247</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=2018220$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Lakämper, Renate</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wunderlich, Dieter</creatorcontrib><title>Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis</title><title>Lingua</title><description>Quechua, which is spoken from the southern parts of Colombia to the northern parts of Chile and Argentina, exhibits a rich verbal morphology. The various dialects of Quechua can be ranked in terms of developmental stages, according to the way in which the inflectional system is organized. Some Peruvian dialects (such as those of Ancash and Ayacucho, belonging to different dialect groups) exhibit remarkable asymmetries in the marking of objects : 1st person objects are marked transparently with separate object and subject affixes, whereas the marking of 2nd person objects is somewhat opaque. We account for this asymmetry by the assumption that the person hierarchy in Quechua is 1 ⪢ 2 ⪢ 3 and that the marking of objects is constrained by the Object-Subject-Constraint (OSC). ‘The object may be marked separately from the subject only if it refers to a person that is higher on the hierarchy of person than the person to which the subject refers’. We will then show that the dialect of Cuzco (Peru) has lost any independent morpheme for 2nd person objects, hence, OSC has become irrelevant. It is this stage that allows reanalysis: the most recent dialects such as those of Potosí (Bolivia) and Santiago del Estero (Argentina) have reinvented a 2nd person object morpheme and have given up OSC; in these dialects a symmetric system of object marking has evolved. We will discuss our findings in terms of two possible types of inflectional systems: one type has a small set of morphemes but additional constraints that restrict the actual combinations of morphemes — and so induce asymmetries, whereas the other type has a greater and more finely differentiated set of morphemes but does not need additional constraints — and so allows more transparency and symmetry in the system. We will also show how the development of the Quechua person system is intermingled with that of the number system. Only if the plural suffix follows the person affixes, as happens in the Quechua II dialects, can plural allomorphs arise that are sensitive to person or that fuse plural and person into one single morpheme, facts that have considerable consequences for the whole system.</description><subject>Constraint-based morphology</subject><subject>Descriptive studies and applied theories</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Morphology</subject><subject>Object agreement</subject><subject>Quechua</subject><issn>0024-3841</issn><issn>1872-6135</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1998</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkMtKxEAQRRtRcHx8gpCFiC6i1Y8kPSuR8QmCirpuOt0Vbc10tCsjuPMj_EK_xIwjbl3V5ty6VYexLQ77HHh5cAsgVC614rtjvQcAHHKxxEZcVyIvuSyW2egPWWVrRE9zSI3HI3Z8jYm6mE1teg7xIQsxu5mhe5zZ7OvjMzvKXBepTzbEPq8toc-mIYapbQP1mY22fadAG2ylsS3h5u9cZ_enJ3eT8_zy6uxicnSZO1lWfY5OSnAohW9E06iqLqQUSvHaC7BeVb5RWhfSYal9XWLtagBbNZUH0AIUl-tsZ7H3JXWvM6TeTAM5bFsbsZuRKbSstFRyAIsF6FJHlLAxL2k4Or0bDmbuzPw4M3MhZqzNjzMjhtz2b4ElZ9sm2egC_YUFcC0EDNjhAsPh2beAyZALGB36kND1xnfhn6JvpdKAqw</recordid><startdate>19980801</startdate><enddate>19980801</enddate><creator>Lakämper, Renate</creator><creator>Wunderlich, Dieter</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19980801</creationdate><title>Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis</title><author>Lakämper, Renate ; Wunderlich, Dieter</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1998</creationdate><topic>Constraint-based morphology</topic><topic>Descriptive studies and applied theories</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Morphology</topic><topic>Object agreement</topic><topic>Quechua</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lakämper, Renate</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wunderlich, Dieter</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Lingua</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lakämper, Renate</au><au>Wunderlich, Dieter</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis</atitle><jtitle>Lingua</jtitle><date>1998-08-01</date><risdate>1998</risdate><volume>105</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>113</spage><epage>148</epage><pages>113-148</pages><issn>0024-3841</issn><eissn>1872-6135</eissn><coden>LINGAO</coden><abstract>Quechua, which is spoken from the southern parts of Colombia to the northern parts of Chile and Argentina, exhibits a rich verbal morphology. The various dialects of Quechua can be ranked in terms of developmental stages, according to the way in which the inflectional system is organized. Some Peruvian dialects (such as those of Ancash and Ayacucho, belonging to different dialect groups) exhibit remarkable asymmetries in the marking of objects : 1st person objects are marked transparently with separate object and subject affixes, whereas the marking of 2nd person objects is somewhat opaque. We account for this asymmetry by the assumption that the person hierarchy in Quechua is 1 ⪢ 2 ⪢ 3 and that the marking of objects is constrained by the Object-Subject-Constraint (OSC). ‘The object may be marked separately from the subject only if it refers to a person that is higher on the hierarchy of person than the person to which the subject refers’. We will then show that the dialect of Cuzco (Peru) has lost any independent morpheme for 2nd person objects, hence, OSC has become irrelevant. It is this stage that allows reanalysis: the most recent dialects such as those of Potosí (Bolivia) and Santiago del Estero (Argentina) have reinvented a 2nd person object morpheme and have given up OSC; in these dialects a symmetric system of object marking has evolved. We will discuss our findings in terms of two possible types of inflectional systems: one type has a small set of morphemes but additional constraints that restrict the actual combinations of morphemes — and so induce asymmetries, whereas the other type has a greater and more finely differentiated set of morphemes but does not need additional constraints — and so allows more transparency and symmetry in the system. We will also show how the development of the Quechua person system is intermingled with that of the number system. Only if the plural suffix follows the person affixes, as happens in the Quechua II dialects, can plural allomorphs arise that are sensitive to person or that fuse plural and person into one single morpheme, facts that have considerable consequences for the whole system.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00010-2</doi><tpages>36</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0024-3841
ispartof Lingua, 1998-08, Vol.105 (3), p.113-148
issn 0024-3841
1872-6135
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_58378343
source ScienceDirect Freedom Collection; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
subjects Constraint-based morphology
Descriptive studies and applied theories
Linguistics
Morphology
Object agreement
Quechua
title Person marking in Quechua — A constraint-based minimalist analysis
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-01T00%3A53%3A34IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Person%20marking%20in%20Quechua%20%E2%80%94%20A%20constraint-based%20minimalist%20analysis&rft.jtitle=Lingua&rft.au=Lak%C3%A4mper,%20Renate&rft.date=1998-08-01&rft.volume=105&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=113&rft.epage=148&rft.pages=113-148&rft.issn=0024-3841&rft.eissn=1872-6135&rft.coden=LINGAO&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00010-2&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E58378343%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c367t-ec330ce32df2ff47b5332441bd20ad47df48853ce68db6ebcb00a7f7d00820413%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=58378343&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true