Loading…
Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project
The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the...
Saved in:
Published in: | Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883 |
container_end_page | 428 |
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 422 |
container_title | Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences |
container_volume | 60 |
creator | HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU MEGURO, KENICHI LEE, EUNJOO KASAI, MARI ISHII, HIROSHI YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI |
description | The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (>6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68716946</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>68716946</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkU2PFCEQhonRuOvqXzBc9NYtNAxNm3gwk_Urq-5hPJOaplhp6Q-hR3f-gT9beqfjXiUkFOGpqpe3CKGclTyvV13JpWQF17wpK8ZUyfimUuXtA3L-7-FhjkUlCi64OiNPUuoYY0Io_piccaW1lLI6J38uncN2pqOjcIMUBkvRHlqY_TjQvOfvSHcRfKCf4YcfbugO03yHWZwx9n5YUUeHMfb58guphRmoGyP9BBMMmJBisBjDkU44TgFf091SFjofPb2OY5cVPCWPHISEz9bzgnx7d7nbfiiuvr7_uH17VbQbUatCgdWOSyugcUoIp2tmtXYbaWtbC5cZuVdqrzhU6FRlXWOxkbIWtWWi0VpckJenulMcfx7yZ0zvU4shZKHjIRmla64aqTKoT2Abx5QiOjNF30M8Gs7MMgXTmcVss5htlimYuymY25z6fO1x2Pdo7xNX2zPwYgUgtRBchKH16Z7TjLNNxTL35sT99gGP_y3AXG-_LJH4C9OHoy0</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>68716946</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><source>Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection</source><creator>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creator><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><description>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (>6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1323-1316</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1440-1819</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x</identifier><identifier>PMID: 16884442</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Melbourne, Australia: Blackwell Publishing Asia</publisher><subject>Aged ; Aged, 80 and over ; Aging - psychology ; aging effect ; Biological and medical sciences ; Education ; educational effect ; Female ; Humans ; Japan ; Male ; Medical sciences ; Neuropsychological Tests ; normal aging ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Psychopathology. Psychiatry ; Reference Values ; Techniques and methods ; Trail Making Test</subject><ispartof>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428</ispartof><rights>2006 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27923,27924</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18010520$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16884442$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MEGURO, KENICHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LEE, EUNJOO</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>KASAI, MARI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>ISHII, HIROSHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><title>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</title><addtitle>Psychiatry Clin Neurosci</addtitle><description>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (>6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</description><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aged, 80 and over</subject><subject>Aging - psychology</subject><subject>aging effect</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Education</subject><subject>educational effect</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Japan</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Neuropsychological Tests</subject><subject>normal aging</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Reference Values</subject><subject>Techniques and methods</subject><subject>Trail Making Test</subject><issn>1323-1316</issn><issn>1440-1819</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqNkU2PFCEQhonRuOvqXzBc9NYtNAxNm3gwk_Urq-5hPJOaplhp6Q-hR3f-gT9beqfjXiUkFOGpqpe3CKGclTyvV13JpWQF17wpK8ZUyfimUuXtA3L-7-FhjkUlCi64OiNPUuoYY0Io_piccaW1lLI6J38uncN2pqOjcIMUBkvRHlqY_TjQvOfvSHcRfKCf4YcfbugO03yHWZwx9n5YUUeHMfb58guphRmoGyP9BBMMmJBisBjDkU44TgFf091SFjofPb2OY5cVPCWPHISEz9bzgnx7d7nbfiiuvr7_uH17VbQbUatCgdWOSyugcUoIp2tmtXYbaWtbC5cZuVdqrzhU6FRlXWOxkbIWtWWi0VpckJenulMcfx7yZ0zvU4shZKHjIRmla64aqTKoT2Abx5QiOjNF30M8Gs7MMgXTmcVss5htlimYuymY25z6fO1x2Pdo7xNX2zPwYgUgtRBchKH16Z7TjLNNxTL35sT99gGP_y3AXG-_LJH4C9OHoy0</recordid><startdate>200608</startdate><enddate>200608</enddate><creator>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creator><creator>MEGURO, KENICHI</creator><creator>LEE, EUNJOO</creator><creator>KASAI, MARI</creator><creator>ISHII, HIROSHI</creator><creator>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Asia</general><general>Blackwell Publishing</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>200608</creationdate><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><author>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aged, 80 and over</topic><topic>Aging - psychology</topic><topic>aging effect</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Education</topic><topic>educational effect</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Japan</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Neuropsychological Tests</topic><topic>normal aging</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Reference Values</topic><topic>Techniques and methods</topic><topic>Trail Making Test</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MEGURO, KENICHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LEE, EUNJOO</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>KASAI, MARI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>ISHII, HIROSHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</au><au>MEGURO, KENICHI</au><au>LEE, EUNJOO</au><au>KASAI, MARI</au><au>ISHII, HIROSHI</au><au>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</atitle><jtitle>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</jtitle><addtitle>Psychiatry Clin Neurosci</addtitle><date>2006-08</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>60</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>422</spage><epage>428</epage><pages>422-428</pages><issn>1323-1316</issn><eissn>1440-1819</eissn><abstract>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (>6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</abstract><cop>Melbourne, Australia</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Asia</pub><pmid>16884442</pmid><doi>10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x</doi><tpages>7</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1323-1316 |
ispartof | Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428 |
issn | 1323-1316 1440-1819 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68716946 |
source | Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection |
subjects | Aged Aged, 80 and over Aging - psychology aging effect Biological and medical sciences Education educational effect Female Humans Japan Male Medical sciences Neuropsychological Tests normal aging Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems Psychomotor Performance - physiology Psychopathology. Psychiatry Reference Values Techniques and methods Trail Making Test |
title | Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-09T08%3A48%3A06IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Effect%20of%20age%20and%20education%20on%20the%20Trail%20Making%20Test%20and%20determination%20of%20normative%20data%20for%20Japanese%20elderly%20people:%20The%20Tajiri%20Project&rft.jtitle=Psychiatry%20and%20clinical%20neurosciences&rft.au=HASHIMOTO,%20RYUSAKU&rft.date=2006-08&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=422&rft.epage=428&rft.pages=422-428&rft.issn=1323-1316&rft.eissn=1440-1819&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E68716946%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=68716946&rft_id=info:pmid/16884442&rfr_iscdi=true |