Loading…

Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project

The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428
Main Authors: HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU, MEGURO, KENICHI, LEE, EUNJOO, KASAI, MARI, ISHII, HIROSHI, YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883
container_end_page 428
container_issue 4
container_start_page 422
container_title Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences
container_volume 60
creator HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU
MEGURO, KENICHI
LEE, EUNJOO
KASAI, MARI
ISHII, HIROSHI
YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI
description The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (>6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68716946</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>68716946</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkU2PFCEQhonRuOvqXzBc9NYtNAxNm3gwk_Urq-5hPJOaplhp6Q-hR3f-gT9beqfjXiUkFOGpqpe3CKGclTyvV13JpWQF17wpK8ZUyfimUuXtA3L-7-FhjkUlCi64OiNPUuoYY0Io_piccaW1lLI6J38uncN2pqOjcIMUBkvRHlqY_TjQvOfvSHcRfKCf4YcfbugO03yHWZwx9n5YUUeHMfb58guphRmoGyP9BBMMmJBisBjDkU44TgFf091SFjofPb2OY5cVPCWPHISEz9bzgnx7d7nbfiiuvr7_uH17VbQbUatCgdWOSyugcUoIp2tmtXYbaWtbC5cZuVdqrzhU6FRlXWOxkbIWtWWi0VpckJenulMcfx7yZ0zvU4shZKHjIRmla64aqTKoT2Abx5QiOjNF30M8Gs7MMgXTmcVss5htlimYuymY25z6fO1x2Pdo7xNX2zPwYgUgtRBchKH16Z7TjLNNxTL35sT99gGP_y3AXG-_LJH4C9OHoy0</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>68716946</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><source>Wiley-Blackwell Read &amp; Publish Collection</source><creator>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creator><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><description>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (&gt;6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1323-1316</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1440-1819</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x</identifier><identifier>PMID: 16884442</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Melbourne, Australia: Blackwell Publishing Asia</publisher><subject>Aged ; Aged, 80 and over ; Aging - psychology ; aging effect ; Biological and medical sciences ; Education ; educational effect ; Female ; Humans ; Japan ; Male ; Medical sciences ; Neuropsychological Tests ; normal aging ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Psychopathology. Psychiatry ; Reference Values ; Techniques and methods ; Trail Making Test</subject><ispartof>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428</ispartof><rights>2006 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27923,27924</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=18010520$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16884442$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MEGURO, KENICHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LEE, EUNJOO</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>KASAI, MARI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>ISHII, HIROSHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><title>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</title><addtitle>Psychiatry Clin Neurosci</addtitle><description>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (&gt;6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</description><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aged, 80 and over</subject><subject>Aging - psychology</subject><subject>aging effect</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Education</subject><subject>educational effect</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Japan</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Neuropsychological Tests</subject><subject>normal aging</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Reference Values</subject><subject>Techniques and methods</subject><subject>Trail Making Test</subject><issn>1323-1316</issn><issn>1440-1819</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqNkU2PFCEQhonRuOvqXzBc9NYtNAxNm3gwk_Urq-5hPJOaplhp6Q-hR3f-gT9beqfjXiUkFOGpqpe3CKGclTyvV13JpWQF17wpK8ZUyfimUuXtA3L-7-FhjkUlCi64OiNPUuoYY0Io_piccaW1lLI6J38uncN2pqOjcIMUBkvRHlqY_TjQvOfvSHcRfKCf4YcfbugO03yHWZwx9n5YUUeHMfb58guphRmoGyP9BBMMmJBisBjDkU44TgFf091SFjofPb2OY5cVPCWPHISEz9bzgnx7d7nbfiiuvr7_uH17VbQbUatCgdWOSyugcUoIp2tmtXYbaWtbC5cZuVdqrzhU6FRlXWOxkbIWtWWi0VpckJenulMcfx7yZ0zvU4shZKHjIRmla64aqTKoT2Abx5QiOjNF30M8Gs7MMgXTmcVss5htlimYuymY25z6fO1x2Pdo7xNX2zPwYgUgtRBchKH16Z7TjLNNxTL35sT99gGP_y3AXG-_LJH4C9OHoy0</recordid><startdate>200608</startdate><enddate>200608</enddate><creator>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creator><creator>MEGURO, KENICHI</creator><creator>LEE, EUNJOO</creator><creator>KASAI, MARI</creator><creator>ISHII, HIROSHI</creator><creator>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Asia</general><general>Blackwell Publishing</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>200608</creationdate><title>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</title><author>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU ; MEGURO, KENICHI ; LEE, EUNJOO ; KASAI, MARI ; ISHII, HIROSHI ; YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aged, 80 and over</topic><topic>Aging - psychology</topic><topic>aging effect</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Education</topic><topic>educational effect</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Japan</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Neuropsychological Tests</topic><topic>normal aging</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Reference Values</topic><topic>Techniques and methods</topic><topic>Trail Making Test</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>MEGURO, KENICHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LEE, EUNJOO</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>KASAI, MARI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>ISHII, HIROSHI</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>HASHIMOTO, RYUSAKU</au><au>MEGURO, KENICHI</au><au>LEE, EUNJOO</au><au>KASAI, MARI</au><au>ISHII, HIROSHI</au><au>YAMAGUCHI, SATOSHI</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project</atitle><jtitle>Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences</jtitle><addtitle>Psychiatry Clin Neurosci</addtitle><date>2006-08</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>60</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>422</spage><epage>428</epage><pages>422-428</pages><issn>1323-1316</issn><eissn>1440-1819</eissn><abstract>The Trail Making Test (TMT) is a common two‐part neuropsychological test, in which visuospatial ability (TMT‐A) and executive function (TMT‐B) are evaluated. Normative data for this test have not been reported for Japanese subjects; therefore, the purpose of the present paper was to investigate the effect of age and education on the TMT in 155 healthy elderly adults with clinical dementia rating 0 (healthy). The participants were classified into three groups based on age (70–74 years, 75–84 years and ≥85 years), and also into three groups based on educational level (6 years, 8 years and ≥10 years). The time to complete TMT‐A and TMT‐B were measured, and the difference in score between TMT‐A and TMT‐B (B–A) and the ratio of the score (B/A) were calculated as indices of executive function. The time for completion of both parts of the TMT increased markedly in the ≥85‐years group. For TMT‐A, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and 8‐years groups, and between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and for TMT‐B, there was a significant difference between the 6‐years and ≥10‐years groups, and between the 8‐years and ≥10‐years groups. The difference and ratio scores increased in the ≥85‐years group, but the educational level did not significantly influence these scores. Our data suggest that cognitive functions evaluated by TMT‐A and TMT‐B are not affected by aging until the subjects are ≥85 years old. For TMT‐A, an educational effect becomes apparent when the population includes poorly educated subjects, but this part of the test is not affected by educational level provided that the subjects have some education (&gt;6 years). The time to complete TMT‐B is affected by educational level, consistent with previous reports. However, when adjusted using the results for TMT‐A [(B‐A) or (B/A)], the educational effect on executive function disappeared. Thus, the effect of educational level on executive function was unclear in normal elderly subjects.</abstract><cop>Melbourne, Australia</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Asia</pub><pmid>16884442</pmid><doi>10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x</doi><tpages>7</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1323-1316
ispartof Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 2006-08, Vol.60 (4), p.422-428
issn 1323-1316
1440-1819
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68716946
source Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection
subjects Aged
Aged, 80 and over
Aging - psychology
aging effect
Biological and medical sciences
Education
educational effect
Female
Humans
Japan
Male
Medical sciences
Neuropsychological Tests
normal aging
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychometrics. Diagnostic aid systems
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Psychopathology. Psychiatry
Reference Values
Techniques and methods
Trail Making Test
title Effect of age and education on the Trail Making Test and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajiri Project
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-09T08%3A48%3A06IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Effect%20of%20age%20and%20education%20on%20the%20Trail%20Making%20Test%20and%20determination%20of%20normative%20data%20for%20Japanese%20elderly%20people:%20The%20Tajiri%20Project&rft.jtitle=Psychiatry%20and%20clinical%20neurosciences&rft.au=HASHIMOTO,%20RYUSAKU&rft.date=2006-08&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=422&rft.epage=428&rft.pages=422-428&rft.issn=1323-1316&rft.eissn=1440-1819&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01526.x&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E68716946%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5376-6ad8f14d3a9f633f870d88f54d7d73f5374b66b61a2ef62df9de944737d039883%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=68716946&rft_id=info:pmid/16884442&rfr_iscdi=true