Loading…

Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training

We studied gaze-shift dynamics during several gaze-shift tasks and during reading, in five subjects with convergence insufficiency (C.I., a diminished ability to converge), and in ten subjects without C.I. Furthermore, we studied the effect of vergence training in order to verify previous claims tha...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Vision research (Oxford) 1999-09, Vol.39 (18), p.3095-3107
Main Authors: van Leeuwen, Anna F, Westen, Marion J, van der Steen, Johannes, de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N, Collewijn, Han
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3
container_end_page 3107
container_issue 18
container_start_page 3095
container_title Vision research (Oxford)
container_volume 39
creator van Leeuwen, Anna F
Westen, Marion J
van der Steen, Johannes
de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N
Collewijn, Han
description We studied gaze-shift dynamics during several gaze-shift tasks and during reading, in five subjects with convergence insufficiency (C.I., a diminished ability to converge), and in ten subjects without C.I. Furthermore, we studied the effect of vergence training in order to verify previous claims that orthoptic exercises can improve vergence performance. We recorded binocular eye movements with the scleral coil technique. Subjects switched fixation between nearby and distant light emitting diodes (LEDs) arranged in isovergence arrays (distances 35 and 130 cm) in a dimly lit room. In both the C.I. and non-C.I. group, two classes of subjects occurred: vergence responders and saccadic responders. During pure vergence tasks, saccadic responders made saccades with no or little vergence; vergence responders made vergence movements with no or small saccadic components. In saccadic responders, fixation of nearby targets was monocular. Subjects with a preferred eye, according to our determination, used the preferred eye. The five C.I. subjects showed idiosyncratic responses with insufficient vergence during most trials. They all had a tendency to alternate fixation between the left and right eye. Vergence–version tasks always elicited larger vergence components than pure vergence tasks. During a reading task, vergence angles were more accurate than during gaze-shifts between LEDs. After the pre-training sessions, nine subjects (one of which had C.I.) practised a pure vergence task three times a day for at least 2 weeks. Vergence amplitudes of four of these subjects were larger after training. We conclude that vergence training can change oculomotor performance. Although C.I. is often associated with abnormal vergence dynamics, there are no typical C.I. vergence dynamics. Unstable monocular preferences may play a role in the aetiology of C.I.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00066-8
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69424732</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0042698999000668</els_id><sourcerecordid>69424732</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkcFuFSEUhomxsdfqI2hYGKOLUYaBGejGmEZbkyYu1DVh4NBLMwNXmKm5PocPXGbujXXnCg585_zw_wi9qMm7mtTt-2-EMFq1Usg3Ur4lhLRtJR6hTS06UfGWtY_R5i9yip7mfFugjlP5BJ3WhWaCdBv051L_hipvvZuw3Qc9epOxDzjP_S2YKeNfftpiHey6ifOE837cTXHMODpsYriDdAPBQGnKs3Pe-FLtz0vphnm9KNwYQzTzoBPeJXCQ1vNl6LQFDM4VpQWbkvbBh5tn6MTpIcPz43qGfnz-9P3iqrr-evnl4uN1ZZiQU8WAdTU1lDV9x1tuGe-s0I2hjvQ9J47VFkwH1jrCZUeN0QQaIFZYzWsqXXOGXh_m7lL8OUOe1OizgWHQAeKcVSsZZV1DC8gPoEkx5_IFtUt-1GmvaqKWONQah1q8VlKqNQ4lSt_Lo8Dcj2D_6Tr4X4BXR0BnoweXdDA-P3CCEykW_Q8HDIobdx6SyqvPYH0q3ikb_X9ecg_hhar4</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>69424732</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training</title><source>Elsevier</source><creator>van Leeuwen, Anna F ; Westen, Marion J ; van der Steen, Johannes ; de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N ; Collewijn, Han</creator><creatorcontrib>van Leeuwen, Anna F ; Westen, Marion J ; van der Steen, Johannes ; de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N ; Collewijn, Han</creatorcontrib><description>We studied gaze-shift dynamics during several gaze-shift tasks and during reading, in five subjects with convergence insufficiency (C.I., a diminished ability to converge), and in ten subjects without C.I. Furthermore, we studied the effect of vergence training in order to verify previous claims that orthoptic exercises can improve vergence performance. We recorded binocular eye movements with the scleral coil technique. Subjects switched fixation between nearby and distant light emitting diodes (LEDs) arranged in isovergence arrays (distances 35 and 130 cm) in a dimly lit room. In both the C.I. and non-C.I. group, two classes of subjects occurred: vergence responders and saccadic responders. During pure vergence tasks, saccadic responders made saccades with no or little vergence; vergence responders made vergence movements with no or small saccadic components. In saccadic responders, fixation of nearby targets was monocular. Subjects with a preferred eye, according to our determination, used the preferred eye. The five C.I. subjects showed idiosyncratic responses with insufficient vergence during most trials. They all had a tendency to alternate fixation between the left and right eye. Vergence–version tasks always elicited larger vergence components than pure vergence tasks. During a reading task, vergence angles were more accurate than during gaze-shifts between LEDs. After the pre-training sessions, nine subjects (one of which had C.I.) practised a pure vergence task three times a day for at least 2 weeks. Vergence amplitudes of four of these subjects were larger after training. We conclude that vergence training can change oculomotor performance. Although C.I. is often associated with abnormal vergence dynamics, there are no typical C.I. vergence dynamics. Unstable monocular preferences may play a role in the aetiology of C.I.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0042-6989</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1878-5646</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00066-8</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10664807</identifier><identifier>CODEN: VISRAM</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford: Elsevier Ltd</publisher><subject>Adult ; Anatomical correlates of behavior ; Behavioral psychophysiology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Convergence insufficiency ; Female ; Fixation, Ocular - physiology ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Human ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Ocular Motility Disorders - physiopathology ; Ocular Motility Disorders - rehabilitation ; Patient Education as Topic ; Preferred eye ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Reading ; Saccades - physiology ; Scleral coils ; Vision, Binocular - physiology ; Vision, Monocular - physiology</subject><ispartof>Vision research (Oxford), 1999-09, Vol.39 (18), p.3095-3107</ispartof><rights>1999 Elsevier Science Ltd</rights><rights>1999 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27915,27916</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=1850982$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10664807$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>van Leeuwen, Anna F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Westen, Marion J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van der Steen, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, Han</creatorcontrib><title>Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training</title><title>Vision research (Oxford)</title><addtitle>Vision Res</addtitle><description>We studied gaze-shift dynamics during several gaze-shift tasks and during reading, in five subjects with convergence insufficiency (C.I., a diminished ability to converge), and in ten subjects without C.I. Furthermore, we studied the effect of vergence training in order to verify previous claims that orthoptic exercises can improve vergence performance. We recorded binocular eye movements with the scleral coil technique. Subjects switched fixation between nearby and distant light emitting diodes (LEDs) arranged in isovergence arrays (distances 35 and 130 cm) in a dimly lit room. In both the C.I. and non-C.I. group, two classes of subjects occurred: vergence responders and saccadic responders. During pure vergence tasks, saccadic responders made saccades with no or little vergence; vergence responders made vergence movements with no or small saccadic components. In saccadic responders, fixation of nearby targets was monocular. Subjects with a preferred eye, according to our determination, used the preferred eye. The five C.I. subjects showed idiosyncratic responses with insufficient vergence during most trials. They all had a tendency to alternate fixation between the left and right eye. Vergence–version tasks always elicited larger vergence components than pure vergence tasks. During a reading task, vergence angles were more accurate than during gaze-shifts between LEDs. After the pre-training sessions, nine subjects (one of which had C.I.) practised a pure vergence task three times a day for at least 2 weeks. Vergence amplitudes of four of these subjects were larger after training. We conclude that vergence training can change oculomotor performance. Although C.I. is often associated with abnormal vergence dynamics, there are no typical C.I. vergence dynamics. Unstable monocular preferences may play a role in the aetiology of C.I.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Anatomical correlates of behavior</subject><subject>Behavioral psychophysiology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Convergence insufficiency</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular - physiology</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Ocular Motility Disorders - physiopathology</subject><subject>Ocular Motility Disorders - rehabilitation</subject><subject>Patient Education as Topic</subject><subject>Preferred eye</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Saccades - physiology</subject><subject>Scleral coils</subject><subject>Vision, Binocular - physiology</subject><subject>Vision, Monocular - physiology</subject><issn>0042-6989</issn><issn>1878-5646</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1999</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkcFuFSEUhomxsdfqI2hYGKOLUYaBGejGmEZbkyYu1DVh4NBLMwNXmKm5PocPXGbujXXnCg585_zw_wi9qMm7mtTt-2-EMFq1Usg3Ur4lhLRtJR6hTS06UfGWtY_R5i9yip7mfFugjlP5BJ3WhWaCdBv051L_hipvvZuw3Qc9epOxDzjP_S2YKeNfftpiHey6ifOE837cTXHMODpsYriDdAPBQGnKs3Pe-FLtz0vphnm9KNwYQzTzoBPeJXCQ1vNl6LQFDM4VpQWbkvbBh5tn6MTpIcPz43qGfnz-9P3iqrr-evnl4uN1ZZiQU8WAdTU1lDV9x1tuGe-s0I2hjvQ9J47VFkwH1jrCZUeN0QQaIFZYzWsqXXOGXh_m7lL8OUOe1OizgWHQAeKcVSsZZV1DC8gPoEkx5_IFtUt-1GmvaqKWONQah1q8VlKqNQ4lSt_Lo8Dcj2D_6Tr4X4BXR0BnoweXdDA-P3CCEykW_Q8HDIobdx6SyqvPYH0q3ikb_X9ecg_hhar4</recordid><startdate>19990901</startdate><enddate>19990901</enddate><creator>van Leeuwen, Anna F</creator><creator>Westen, Marion J</creator><creator>van der Steen, Johannes</creator><creator>de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N</creator><creator>Collewijn, Han</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><scope>6I.</scope><scope>AAFTH</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19990901</creationdate><title>Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training</title><author>van Leeuwen, Anna F ; Westen, Marion J ; van der Steen, Johannes ; de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N ; Collewijn, Han</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1999</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Anatomical correlates of behavior</topic><topic>Behavioral psychophysiology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Convergence insufficiency</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular - physiology</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Ocular Motility Disorders - physiopathology</topic><topic>Ocular Motility Disorders - rehabilitation</topic><topic>Patient Education as Topic</topic><topic>Preferred eye</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Saccades - physiology</topic><topic>Scleral coils</topic><topic>Vision, Binocular - physiology</topic><topic>Vision, Monocular - physiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>van Leeuwen, Anna F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Westen, Marion J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>van der Steen, Johannes</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, Han</creatorcontrib><collection>ScienceDirect Open Access Titles</collection><collection>Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Vision research (Oxford)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>van Leeuwen, Anna F</au><au>Westen, Marion J</au><au>van der Steen, Johannes</au><au>de Faber, Jan-Tjeerd H.N</au><au>Collewijn, Han</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training</atitle><jtitle>Vision research (Oxford)</jtitle><addtitle>Vision Res</addtitle><date>1999-09-01</date><risdate>1999</risdate><volume>39</volume><issue>18</issue><spage>3095</spage><epage>3107</epage><pages>3095-3107</pages><issn>0042-6989</issn><eissn>1878-5646</eissn><coden>VISRAM</coden><abstract>We studied gaze-shift dynamics during several gaze-shift tasks and during reading, in five subjects with convergence insufficiency (C.I., a diminished ability to converge), and in ten subjects without C.I. Furthermore, we studied the effect of vergence training in order to verify previous claims that orthoptic exercises can improve vergence performance. We recorded binocular eye movements with the scleral coil technique. Subjects switched fixation between nearby and distant light emitting diodes (LEDs) arranged in isovergence arrays (distances 35 and 130 cm) in a dimly lit room. In both the C.I. and non-C.I. group, two classes of subjects occurred: vergence responders and saccadic responders. During pure vergence tasks, saccadic responders made saccades with no or little vergence; vergence responders made vergence movements with no or small saccadic components. In saccadic responders, fixation of nearby targets was monocular. Subjects with a preferred eye, according to our determination, used the preferred eye. The five C.I. subjects showed idiosyncratic responses with insufficient vergence during most trials. They all had a tendency to alternate fixation between the left and right eye. Vergence–version tasks always elicited larger vergence components than pure vergence tasks. During a reading task, vergence angles were more accurate than during gaze-shifts between LEDs. After the pre-training sessions, nine subjects (one of which had C.I.) practised a pure vergence task three times a day for at least 2 weeks. Vergence amplitudes of four of these subjects were larger after training. We conclude that vergence training can change oculomotor performance. Although C.I. is often associated with abnormal vergence dynamics, there are no typical C.I. vergence dynamics. Unstable monocular preferences may play a role in the aetiology of C.I.</abstract><cop>Oxford</cop><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><pmid>10664807</pmid><doi>10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00066-8</doi><tpages>13</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0042-6989
ispartof Vision research (Oxford), 1999-09, Vol.39 (18), p.3095-3107
issn 0042-6989
1878-5646
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69424732
source Elsevier
subjects Adult
Anatomical correlates of behavior
Behavioral psychophysiology
Biological and medical sciences
Convergence insufficiency
Female
Fixation, Ocular - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Human
Humans
Male
Middle Aged
Ocular Motility Disorders - physiopathology
Ocular Motility Disorders - rehabilitation
Patient Education as Topic
Preferred eye
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Reading
Saccades - physiology
Scleral coils
Vision, Binocular - physiology
Vision, Monocular - physiology
title Gaze-shift dynamics in subjects with and without symptoms of convergence insufficiency: influence of monocular preference and the effect of training
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-14T23%3A29%3A28IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Gaze-shift%20dynamics%20in%20subjects%20with%20and%20without%20symptoms%20of%20convergence%20insufficiency:%20influence%20of%20monocular%20preference%20and%20the%20effect%20of%20training&rft.jtitle=Vision%20research%20(Oxford)&rft.au=van%20Leeuwen,%20Anna%20F&rft.date=1999-09-01&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=18&rft.spage=3095&rft.epage=3107&rft.pages=3095-3107&rft.issn=0042-6989&rft.eissn=1878-5646&rft.coden=VISRAM&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00066-8&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E69424732%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c489t-4e4712c243b7565d457d8a3c2f0bb50f41dec7eddf05972cca0e3e0d8da5129f3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=69424732&rft_id=info:pmid/10664807&rfr_iscdi=true