Loading…

Interception of targets using brief directional cues

There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Experimental brain research 2004-05, Vol.156 (1), p.94-103
Main Authors: MROTEK, Leigh A, FLANDERS, Martha, SOECHTING, John F
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-c61d99be86734f89bffd5f46fa977fbc81fccedb37858205cb3fe2120bba3bd33
cites
container_end_page 103
container_issue 1
container_start_page 94
container_title Experimental brain research
container_volume 156
creator MROTEK, Leigh A
FLANDERS, Martha
SOECHTING, John F
description There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the time required to obtain a reliable estimate of the direction of target motion. Subjects were asked to indicate the final direction of a moving target, which abruptly changed direction and shortly thereafter disappeared, by pointing to its expected emergence at the boundary of an occlusion. Subjects made small but consistent errors that overestimated the target's change in direction. These errors depended little on the amount of time the target was visible (ranging from 50 to 400 ms) after it changed direction. Pointing direction was strongly correlated with gaze, which was dominated by a saccade initiated shortly after the target changed direction. The pointing errors were explained by the fact that the saccade always intercepted the (occluded) target, but then continued in the same direction toward the boundary of the occlusion. The analysis reveals that target direction was estimated accurately even at the shortest viewing time.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/s00221-003-1764-2
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_71844068</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>17753601</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-c61d99be86734f89bffd5f46fa977fbc81fccedb37858205cb3fe2120bba3bd33</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE1LxDAQhoMo7vrxA7xIEfRWnUnSJD3K4hcIXvQckjSRLt12TdqD_96UXRC8eBoGnvcd5iHkAuEWAeRdAqAUSwBWohS8pAdkiZzREhHEIVkCIC-5wnpBTlJazyuTcEwWyCWlEnBJ-Es_-uj8dmyHvhhCMZr46cdUTKntPwsbWx-Kpo3ezYDpCjf5dEaOgumSP9_PU_Lx-PC-ei5f355eVvevpWOqGksnsKlr65WQjAdV2xCaKnARTC1lsE5hcM43lklVKQqVsyx4ihSsNcw2jJ2Sm13vNg5f-e6oN21yvutM74cpaYmKcxDqXxClrJgAzODVH3A9TDH_lTTFCrMTBhnCHeTikFL0QW9juzHxWyPoWbzeiddZvJ7Fa5ozl_viyW5885vYm87A9R4wyZkuRNO7Nv1ylahE_oT9AEuKigo</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>215127030</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Interception of targets using brief directional cues</title><source>Springer Nature</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><creator>MROTEK, Leigh A ; FLANDERS, Martha ; SOECHTING, John F</creator><creatorcontrib>MROTEK, Leigh A ; FLANDERS, Martha ; SOECHTING, John F</creatorcontrib><description>There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the time required to obtain a reliable estimate of the direction of target motion. Subjects were asked to indicate the final direction of a moving target, which abruptly changed direction and shortly thereafter disappeared, by pointing to its expected emergence at the boundary of an occlusion. Subjects made small but consistent errors that overestimated the target's change in direction. These errors depended little on the amount of time the target was visible (ranging from 50 to 400 ms) after it changed direction. Pointing direction was strongly correlated with gaze, which was dominated by a saccade initiated shortly after the target changed direction. The pointing errors were explained by the fact that the saccade always intercepted the (occluded) target, but then continued in the same direction toward the boundary of the occlusion. The analysis reveals that target direction was estimated accurately even at the shortest viewing time.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-003-1764-2</identifier><identifier>PMID: 14722701</identifier><identifier>CODEN: EXBRAP</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin: Springer</publisher><subject>Biological and medical sciences ; Cues ; Fixation, Ocular - physiology ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Hand - physiology ; Humans ; Medical sciences ; Motion Perception - physiology ; Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration ; Ophthalmology ; Photic Stimulation - methods ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Saccades - physiology ; Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs ; Vision disorders</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2004-05, Vol.156 (1), p.94-103</ispartof><rights>2004 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag 2004</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-c61d99be86734f89bffd5f46fa977fbc81fccedb37858205cb3fe2120bba3bd33</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/215127030/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/215127030?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21394,27924,27925,33611,33612,43733,74221</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=15656683$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14722701$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>MROTEK, Leigh A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>FLANDERS, Martha</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SOECHTING, John F</creatorcontrib><title>Interception of targets using brief directional cues</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the time required to obtain a reliable estimate of the direction of target motion. Subjects were asked to indicate the final direction of a moving target, which abruptly changed direction and shortly thereafter disappeared, by pointing to its expected emergence at the boundary of an occlusion. Subjects made small but consistent errors that overestimated the target's change in direction. These errors depended little on the amount of time the target was visible (ranging from 50 to 400 ms) after it changed direction. Pointing direction was strongly correlated with gaze, which was dominated by a saccade initiated shortly after the target changed direction. The pointing errors were explained by the fact that the saccade always intercepted the (occluded) target, but then continued in the same direction toward the boundary of the occlusion. The analysis reveals that target direction was estimated accurately even at the shortest viewing time.</description><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular - physiology</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Hand - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Motion Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration</subject><subject>Ophthalmology</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation - methods</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Saccades - physiology</subject><subject>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><subject>Vision disorders</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2004</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkE1LxDAQhoMo7vrxA7xIEfRWnUnSJD3K4hcIXvQckjSRLt12TdqD_96UXRC8eBoGnvcd5iHkAuEWAeRdAqAUSwBWohS8pAdkiZzREhHEIVkCIC-5wnpBTlJazyuTcEwWyCWlEnBJ-Es_-uj8dmyHvhhCMZr46cdUTKntPwsbWx-Kpo3ezYDpCjf5dEaOgumSP9_PU_Lx-PC-ei5f355eVvevpWOqGksnsKlr65WQjAdV2xCaKnARTC1lsE5hcM43lklVKQqVsyx4ihSsNcw2jJ2Sm13vNg5f-e6oN21yvutM74cpaYmKcxDqXxClrJgAzODVH3A9TDH_lTTFCrMTBhnCHeTikFL0QW9juzHxWyPoWbzeiddZvJ7Fa5ozl_viyW5885vYm87A9R4wyZkuRNO7Nv1ylahE_oT9AEuKigo</recordid><startdate>20040501</startdate><enddate>20040501</enddate><creator>MROTEK, Leigh A</creator><creator>FLANDERS, Martha</creator><creator>SOECHTING, John F</creator><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20040501</creationdate><title>Interception of targets using brief directional cues</title><author>MROTEK, Leigh A ; FLANDERS, Martha ; SOECHTING, John F</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-c61d99be86734f89bffd5f46fa977fbc81fccedb37858205cb3fe2120bba3bd33</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2004</creationdate><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular - physiology</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Hand - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Motion Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration</topic><topic>Ophthalmology</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation - methods</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Saccades - physiology</topic><topic>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</topic><topic>Vision disorders</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>MROTEK, Leigh A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>FLANDERS, Martha</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SOECHTING, John F</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium &amp; Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Nursing and Allied Health Source</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health and Medical</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>MROTEK, Leigh A</au><au>FLANDERS, Martha</au><au>SOECHTING, John F</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Interception of targets using brief directional cues</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2004-05-01</date><risdate>2004</risdate><volume>156</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>94</spage><epage>103</epage><pages>94-103</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><coden>EXBRAP</coden><abstract>There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the time required to obtain a reliable estimate of the direction of target motion. Subjects were asked to indicate the final direction of a moving target, which abruptly changed direction and shortly thereafter disappeared, by pointing to its expected emergence at the boundary of an occlusion. Subjects made small but consistent errors that overestimated the target's change in direction. These errors depended little on the amount of time the target was visible (ranging from 50 to 400 ms) after it changed direction. Pointing direction was strongly correlated with gaze, which was dominated by a saccade initiated shortly after the target changed direction. The pointing errors were explained by the fact that the saccade always intercepted the (occluded) target, but then continued in the same direction toward the boundary of the occlusion. The analysis reveals that target direction was estimated accurately even at the shortest viewing time.</abstract><cop>Berlin</cop><pub>Springer</pub><pmid>14722701</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-003-1764-2</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0014-4819
ispartof Experimental brain research, 2004-05, Vol.156 (1), p.94-103
issn 0014-4819
1432-1106
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_71844068
source Springer Nature; Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)
subjects Biological and medical sciences
Cues
Fixation, Ocular - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Hand - physiology
Humans
Medical sciences
Motion Perception - physiology
Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration
Ophthalmology
Photic Stimulation - methods
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time - physiology
Saccades - physiology
Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs
Vision disorders
title Interception of targets using brief directional cues
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T12%3A43%3A18IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Interception%20of%20targets%20using%20brief%20directional%20cues&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=MROTEK,%20Leigh%20A&rft.date=2004-05-01&rft.volume=156&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=94&rft.epage=103&rft.pages=94-103&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.coden=EXBRAP&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-003-1764-2&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E17753601%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c385t-c61d99be86734f89bffd5f46fa977fbc81fccedb37858205cb3fe2120bba3bd33%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=215127030&rft_id=info:pmid/14722701&rfr_iscdi=true