Loading…

Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items

Cognitive experimentation suggests that at any single instant only three or four items (“chunks”) are simultaneously prominent as a working memory (WM) trace, if we disregard the rehearsal component of WM. The reason for small WM capacity may concern combinatorial manageability. How might the neural...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Brain Research Bulletin 2003-04, Vol.60 (1), p.25-42
Main Author: Glassman, Robert B
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923
container_end_page 42
container_issue 1
container_start_page 25
container_title Brain Research Bulletin
container_volume 60
creator Glassman, Robert B
description Cognitive experimentation suggests that at any single instant only three or four items (“chunks”) are simultaneously prominent as a working memory (WM) trace, if we disregard the rehearsal component of WM. The reason for small WM capacity may concern combinatorial manageability. How might the neural representations of these few coactive chunks occupy a spatially distributed set of areas of the sheet-like cortex, while providing both order and flexibility to associate items in WM? Each attribute of each simultaneously active WM item must have broad access to the representational facilities of the cortical sheet, comprising tens of thousands of modular “cortical columns.” The two hypothesized neural levels of WM during any moment of cognition comprise (a) “binding” together of many distributed attribute representations within each respective WM chunk, and (b) combinatorial play among three or four WM chunk-representations. Anatomical and functional evidence of cortical unity through its depth suggests that cortex may be viewed as essentially planar in its distribution of activations. Thus, a moment’s WM is hypothesized here to reside in myriad activated cortical planar “patches,” each subdivided into up to four amoeboid “subpatches.” Two different lines of topological reasoning suggest orderly associations of such representations. (1) The four-color principle of map topology, and the related K 4 is planar theorem of graph theory, imply that if a small cortical area is dynamically subdivided into no more than four, discretely bounded planar subareas, then each such segment has ample free access to each of the others. (2) A hypothetical alternative to such associative adjacency of simultaneously active cortical representations of chunk-attributes is associative overlap, whereby, in dense cortical neuropil, activated subpatches behave like Venn diagrams of intersecting sets. As the number of Venn-like coactive subpatches within a patch increases, maintaining ad hoc associativity among all combinations requires exponentially proliferating intersections. Beyond four, serpentine subpatch shapes are required, which could easily lead to pathologies of omission or commission. As hypothesized by many researchers, the binding of the widely distributed cortical modules that represent a given chunk may involve synchrony or coherence of a single EEG frequency. Elsewhere, I have conjectured that such a binding frequency for a single chunk may bear a harmonic relationship w
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00030-3
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_73263183</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0361923003000303</els_id><sourcerecordid>73263183</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE2P1SAUhonROHdGf4KGjWZcVKG0QFcTM_ErmcSF45pQergXpaUCVbv2j0vn3jhLV4ecPO_hzYPQM0peU0L5my-EcVp1NSOXhL0ihDBSsQdoR6VgVS0a8RDt_iFn6DylbwXisuWP0RmtRd3KjuzQn9swBx_2K9bTgPdRzwecDxBiWcyzdzDgHLAJMTujfYF0DuOKR73iA_gZw-_ZazfhXyF-d9MejzBuWaNnbVxesQ2x3IsAuDxsWCJOblx81hOEJWGXYUxP0COrfYKnp3mBvr5_d3v9sbr5_OHT9dubyrCO5oo1nNc95a1tBqObnhrGuwGY5bIXsmGN1owx2_W658a2VnAYJAgQRIraFg0X6OXx7hzDjwVSVqNLBrw_llGC1ZxRyQrYHkETQ0oRrJqjG3VcFSVqs6_u7KtNrSJM3dlXW-756YOlH2G4T510F-DFCdCp6LRRT8ale64RXEq2Nb06clB0_HQQVTIOJgODi2CyGoL7T5W_x96kEw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>73263183</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items</title><source>ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Glassman, Robert B</creator><creatorcontrib>Glassman, Robert B</creatorcontrib><description>Cognitive experimentation suggests that at any single instant only three or four items (“chunks”) are simultaneously prominent as a working memory (WM) trace, if we disregard the rehearsal component of WM. The reason for small WM capacity may concern combinatorial manageability. How might the neural representations of these few coactive chunks occupy a spatially distributed set of areas of the sheet-like cortex, while providing both order and flexibility to associate items in WM? Each attribute of each simultaneously active WM item must have broad access to the representational facilities of the cortical sheet, comprising tens of thousands of modular “cortical columns.” The two hypothesized neural levels of WM during any moment of cognition comprise (a) “binding” together of many distributed attribute representations within each respective WM chunk, and (b) combinatorial play among three or four WM chunk-representations. Anatomical and functional evidence of cortical unity through its depth suggests that cortex may be viewed as essentially planar in its distribution of activations. Thus, a moment’s WM is hypothesized here to reside in myriad activated cortical planar “patches,” each subdivided into up to four amoeboid “subpatches.” Two different lines of topological reasoning suggest orderly associations of such representations. (1) The four-color principle of map topology, and the related K 4 is planar theorem of graph theory, imply that if a small cortical area is dynamically subdivided into no more than four, discretely bounded planar subareas, then each such segment has ample free access to each of the others. (2) A hypothetical alternative to such associative adjacency of simultaneously active cortical representations of chunk-attributes is associative overlap, whereby, in dense cortical neuropil, activated subpatches behave like Venn diagrams of intersecting sets. As the number of Venn-like coactive subpatches within a patch increases, maintaining ad hoc associativity among all combinations requires exponentially proliferating intersections. Beyond four, serpentine subpatch shapes are required, which could easily lead to pathologies of omission or commission. As hypothesized by many researchers, the binding of the widely distributed cortical modules that represent a given chunk may involve synchrony or coherence of a single EEG frequency. Elsewhere, I have conjectured that such a binding frequency for a single chunk may bear a harmonic relationship with the additional EEG frequencies that are simultaneously binding the other WM chunks. Other possible mechanisms of binding have also been hypothesized. Whatever the mechanism, the many attributes of a moment’s complement of three or four WM chunks must generally have an accidental relationship with the spatial distribution of the cortical feature analyzers that must be activated to represent those attributes. Therefore, the cortex may need, and have, comprehensive anatomical connections of each of its modules for representing an attribute (or of small redundant module groupings) with every other. If such whole–part cortico-cortical connections are somehow exploited not only to fully represent each cognitive chunk in its bound-together attributes, but also to bring the major business of intensive WM information processing down to the level of local circuits, in the sorts of topological patterning hypothesized here, there may be two adaptive results: (1) Time and other economies would be achieved in the reduction of activity in distant cortico-cortical connections to lower-energy global orchestration, or binding processes. (2) The piecemeal local topological limit to four subpatches would be writ large, across the entire cortex, preventing an unconstrained combinatorial explosion of associations among all attributes of all three or four simultaneously active chunks. Such hypothetical convergence to foci in local subpatch interactions might take place primarily in association cortex, and/or it might involve temporary shifts in response properties in some cortical subpatches. Quantitative studies of the densely packed cortical fine structure, by Braitenberg and Schüz, and others, seem potentially consistent with this vision of cortical function in cognition.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0361-9230</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-2747</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00030-3</identifier><identifier>PMID: 12725890</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BRBUDU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York, NY: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Animals ; Association ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cerebral Cortex - anatomy &amp; histology ; Cerebral Cortex - physiology ; Cognition - physiology ; Column ; Combinatorial ; Cortex ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Memory, Short-Term - physiology ; Models, Neurological ; Module ; Nerve Net ; Planar</subject><ispartof>Brain Research Bulletin, 2003-04, Vol.60 (1), p.25-42</ispartof><rights>2003 Elsevier Science Inc.</rights><rights>2003 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>313,314,780,784,792,27922,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=14768832$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12725890$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Glassman, Robert B</creatorcontrib><title>Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items</title><title>Brain Research Bulletin</title><addtitle>Brain Res Bull</addtitle><description>Cognitive experimentation suggests that at any single instant only three or four items (“chunks”) are simultaneously prominent as a working memory (WM) trace, if we disregard the rehearsal component of WM. The reason for small WM capacity may concern combinatorial manageability. How might the neural representations of these few coactive chunks occupy a spatially distributed set of areas of the sheet-like cortex, while providing both order and flexibility to associate items in WM? Each attribute of each simultaneously active WM item must have broad access to the representational facilities of the cortical sheet, comprising tens of thousands of modular “cortical columns.” The two hypothesized neural levels of WM during any moment of cognition comprise (a) “binding” together of many distributed attribute representations within each respective WM chunk, and (b) combinatorial play among three or four WM chunk-representations. Anatomical and functional evidence of cortical unity through its depth suggests that cortex may be viewed as essentially planar in its distribution of activations. Thus, a moment’s WM is hypothesized here to reside in myriad activated cortical planar “patches,” each subdivided into up to four amoeboid “subpatches.” Two different lines of topological reasoning suggest orderly associations of such representations. (1) The four-color principle of map topology, and the related K 4 is planar theorem of graph theory, imply that if a small cortical area is dynamically subdivided into no more than four, discretely bounded planar subareas, then each such segment has ample free access to each of the others. (2) A hypothetical alternative to such associative adjacency of simultaneously active cortical representations of chunk-attributes is associative overlap, whereby, in dense cortical neuropil, activated subpatches behave like Venn diagrams of intersecting sets. As the number of Venn-like coactive subpatches within a patch increases, maintaining ad hoc associativity among all combinations requires exponentially proliferating intersections. Beyond four, serpentine subpatch shapes are required, which could easily lead to pathologies of omission or commission. As hypothesized by many researchers, the binding of the widely distributed cortical modules that represent a given chunk may involve synchrony or coherence of a single EEG frequency. Elsewhere, I have conjectured that such a binding frequency for a single chunk may bear a harmonic relationship with the additional EEG frequencies that are simultaneously binding the other WM chunks. Other possible mechanisms of binding have also been hypothesized. Whatever the mechanism, the many attributes of a moment’s complement of three or four WM chunks must generally have an accidental relationship with the spatial distribution of the cortical feature analyzers that must be activated to represent those attributes. Therefore, the cortex may need, and have, comprehensive anatomical connections of each of its modules for representing an attribute (or of small redundant module groupings) with every other. If such whole–part cortico-cortical connections are somehow exploited not only to fully represent each cognitive chunk in its bound-together attributes, but also to bring the major business of intensive WM information processing down to the level of local circuits, in the sorts of topological patterning hypothesized here, there may be two adaptive results: (1) Time and other economies would be achieved in the reduction of activity in distant cortico-cortical connections to lower-energy global orchestration, or binding processes. (2) The piecemeal local topological limit to four subpatches would be writ large, across the entire cortex, preventing an unconstrained combinatorial explosion of associations among all attributes of all three or four simultaneously active chunks. Such hypothetical convergence to foci in local subpatch interactions might take place primarily in association cortex, and/or it might involve temporary shifts in response properties in some cortical subpatches. Quantitative studies of the densely packed cortical fine structure, by Braitenberg and Schüz, and others, seem potentially consistent with this vision of cortical function in cognition.</description><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Association</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cerebral Cortex - anatomy &amp; histology</subject><subject>Cerebral Cortex - physiology</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Column</subject><subject>Combinatorial</subject><subject>Cortex</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Memory, Short-Term - physiology</subject><subject>Models, Neurological</subject><subject>Module</subject><subject>Nerve Net</subject><subject>Planar</subject><issn>0361-9230</issn><issn>1873-2747</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2003</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkE2P1SAUhonROHdGf4KGjWZcVKG0QFcTM_ErmcSF45pQergXpaUCVbv2j0vn3jhLV4ecPO_hzYPQM0peU0L5my-EcVp1NSOXhL0ihDBSsQdoR6VgVS0a8RDt_iFn6DylbwXisuWP0RmtRd3KjuzQn9swBx_2K9bTgPdRzwecDxBiWcyzdzDgHLAJMTujfYF0DuOKR73iA_gZw-_ZazfhXyF-d9MejzBuWaNnbVxesQ2x3IsAuDxsWCJOblx81hOEJWGXYUxP0COrfYKnp3mBvr5_d3v9sbr5_OHT9dubyrCO5oo1nNc95a1tBqObnhrGuwGY5bIXsmGN1owx2_W658a2VnAYJAgQRIraFg0X6OXx7hzDjwVSVqNLBrw_llGC1ZxRyQrYHkETQ0oRrJqjG3VcFSVqs6_u7KtNrSJM3dlXW-756YOlH2G4T510F-DFCdCp6LRRT8ale64RXEq2Nb06clB0_HQQVTIOJgODi2CyGoL7T5W_x96kEw</recordid><startdate>20030415</startdate><enddate>20030415</enddate><creator>Glassman, Robert B</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20030415</creationdate><title>Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items</title><author>Glassman, Robert B</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2003</creationdate><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Association</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cerebral Cortex - anatomy &amp; histology</topic><topic>Cerebral Cortex - physiology</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Column</topic><topic>Combinatorial</topic><topic>Cortex</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Memory, Short-Term - physiology</topic><topic>Models, Neurological</topic><topic>Module</topic><topic>Nerve Net</topic><topic>Planar</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Glassman, Robert B</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Brain Research Bulletin</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Glassman, Robert B</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items</atitle><jtitle>Brain Research Bulletin</jtitle><addtitle>Brain Res Bull</addtitle><date>2003-04-15</date><risdate>2003</risdate><volume>60</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>25</spage><epage>42</epage><pages>25-42</pages><issn>0361-9230</issn><eissn>1873-2747</eissn><coden>BRBUDU</coden><abstract>Cognitive experimentation suggests that at any single instant only three or four items (“chunks”) are simultaneously prominent as a working memory (WM) trace, if we disregard the rehearsal component of WM. The reason for small WM capacity may concern combinatorial manageability. How might the neural representations of these few coactive chunks occupy a spatially distributed set of areas of the sheet-like cortex, while providing both order and flexibility to associate items in WM? Each attribute of each simultaneously active WM item must have broad access to the representational facilities of the cortical sheet, comprising tens of thousands of modular “cortical columns.” The two hypothesized neural levels of WM during any moment of cognition comprise (a) “binding” together of many distributed attribute representations within each respective WM chunk, and (b) combinatorial play among three or four WM chunk-representations. Anatomical and functional evidence of cortical unity through its depth suggests that cortex may be viewed as essentially planar in its distribution of activations. Thus, a moment’s WM is hypothesized here to reside in myriad activated cortical planar “patches,” each subdivided into up to four amoeboid “subpatches.” Two different lines of topological reasoning suggest orderly associations of such representations. (1) The four-color principle of map topology, and the related K 4 is planar theorem of graph theory, imply that if a small cortical area is dynamically subdivided into no more than four, discretely bounded planar subareas, then each such segment has ample free access to each of the others. (2) A hypothetical alternative to such associative adjacency of simultaneously active cortical representations of chunk-attributes is associative overlap, whereby, in dense cortical neuropil, activated subpatches behave like Venn diagrams of intersecting sets. As the number of Venn-like coactive subpatches within a patch increases, maintaining ad hoc associativity among all combinations requires exponentially proliferating intersections. Beyond four, serpentine subpatch shapes are required, which could easily lead to pathologies of omission or commission. As hypothesized by many researchers, the binding of the widely distributed cortical modules that represent a given chunk may involve synchrony or coherence of a single EEG frequency. Elsewhere, I have conjectured that such a binding frequency for a single chunk may bear a harmonic relationship with the additional EEG frequencies that are simultaneously binding the other WM chunks. Other possible mechanisms of binding have also been hypothesized. Whatever the mechanism, the many attributes of a moment’s complement of three or four WM chunks must generally have an accidental relationship with the spatial distribution of the cortical feature analyzers that must be activated to represent those attributes. Therefore, the cortex may need, and have, comprehensive anatomical connections of each of its modules for representing an attribute (or of small redundant module groupings) with every other. If such whole–part cortico-cortical connections are somehow exploited not only to fully represent each cognitive chunk in its bound-together attributes, but also to bring the major business of intensive WM information processing down to the level of local circuits, in the sorts of topological patterning hypothesized here, there may be two adaptive results: (1) Time and other economies would be achieved in the reduction of activity in distant cortico-cortical connections to lower-energy global orchestration, or binding processes. (2) The piecemeal local topological limit to four subpatches would be writ large, across the entire cortex, preventing an unconstrained combinatorial explosion of associations among all attributes of all three or four simultaneously active chunks. Such hypothetical convergence to foci in local subpatch interactions might take place primarily in association cortex, and/or it might involve temporary shifts in response properties in some cortical subpatches. Quantitative studies of the densely packed cortical fine structure, by Braitenberg and Schüz, and others, seem potentially consistent with this vision of cortical function in cognition.</abstract><cop>New York, NY</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>12725890</pmid><doi>10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00030-3</doi><tpages>18</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0361-9230
ispartof Brain Research Bulletin, 2003-04, Vol.60 (1), p.25-42
issn 0361-9230
1873-2747
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_73263183
source ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Animals
Association
Biological and medical sciences
Cerebral Cortex - anatomy & histology
Cerebral Cortex - physiology
Cognition - physiology
Column
Combinatorial
Cortex
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Memory, Short-Term - physiology
Models, Neurological
Module
Nerve Net
Planar
title Topology and graph theory applied to cortical anatomy may help explain working memory capacity for three or four simultaneous items
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T05%3A41%3A03IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Topology%20and%20graph%20theory%20applied%20to%20cortical%20anatomy%20may%20help%20explain%20working%20memory%20capacity%20for%20three%20or%20four%20simultaneous%20items&rft.jtitle=Brain%20Research%20Bulletin&rft.au=Glassman,%20Robert%20B&rft.date=2003-04-15&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=25&rft.epage=42&rft.pages=25-42&rft.issn=0361-9230&rft.eissn=1873-2747&rft.coden=BRBUDU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00030-3&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E73263183%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c391t-34662b165f4dca4b1c369de3f68b78434aa333f9bab6cf5f76ed8e7e70872f923%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=73263183&rft_id=info:pmid/12725890&rfr_iscdi=true