Loading…

Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements: The Role of the Kinetic Visual Feedback System

Vision has long been considered as a single feedback system providing information about the static and the dynamic features of motor behaviors and of the environment where they take place. However, recent models for oculomanual movements have included multichanneling of visual cues ( Goodale & M...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 1993-12, Vol.47 (4), p.678-696
Main Authors: BLOUIN, J, TEASDALE, N, BARD, C, FLEURY, M
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a485t-cf6530c91a4d8439a99f41dbd36d4bfab1bcd9f3ddf1e8ec1147e6a5516be39b3
cites
container_end_page 696
container_issue 4
container_start_page 678
container_title Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology
container_volume 47
creator BLOUIN, J
TEASDALE, N
BARD, C
FLEURY, M
description Vision has long been considered as a single feedback system providing information about the static and the dynamic features of motor behaviors and of the environment where they take place. However, recent models for oculomanual movements have included multichanneling of visual cues ( Goodale & Milner, 1992 ; Jeannerod, 1981 ; 1984 ; Paillard, 1980 , 1982 ). According to Paillard's model, a kinetic system, mostly sensitive to dynamic cues, provides directional information of the movement in the rapid distance-covering phase, and a static system, highly sensitive to position cues, provides positional signal errors. The present experiment gives kinematic evidence for the significant contribution of vision during the initial phase of rapid pointing movements when this phase is under the control of the kinetic channel. Movements having directional requirements were more accurate (directional precision) when vision of the initial portion of the trajectory was available. Times-to-peak acceleration and velocity were all shorter and their respective amplitudes were generally higher when vision was available for the first third of the trajectory than when it was not. Further, vision of the entire trajectory did not yield better precision then when vision was available for the initial phase of the movements only. Overall, the data support the existence of two corrective visual feedback systems.
doi_str_mv 10.1037/h0078869
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_76262625</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>57384993</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a485t-cf6530c91a4d8439a99f41dbd36d4bfab1bcd9f3ddf1e8ec1147e6a5516be39b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0W1r1TAYBuAgk7lNYX9AKJsMmdTlad4_jrOpg4kg-jmkecGOtumSdrB_bw7nOHHgRggJPBc3JDdCh4A_Aibi7BfGQkquXqA9kELWolF4p9xB8bpseIX2c77BuGAKu2hXQkMbBXvo9KJL3s5dHE1freI4p9hXMVTfzdS56jwN1dd45wc_zvk1ehlMn_2b7XmAfn66_LH6Ul9_-3y1Or-uDZVsrm3gjGCrwFAnKVFGqUDBtY5wR9tgWmitU4E4F8BLbwGo8NwwBrz1RLXkAJ1scqcUbxefZz102fq-N6OPS9aCN-vFnoVMEEmVIgUePYI3cUnlxVlzoISwRjRPoQZjohhTsqDj_yEoX44xV6CKer9RNsWckw96St1g0r0GrNd96T99Ffp2G7i0g3cPcFtQmb_bzk22pg_JjLbLD4xiySTnhX3YMDMZPeV7a9Lc2d5nu6RUytPWT5oKTTUX8m_oY_0v-w0SZ7Sp</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>200395598</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements: The Role of the Kinetic Visual Feedback System</title><source>APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection</source><creator>BLOUIN, J ; TEASDALE, N ; BARD, C ; FLEURY, M</creator><contributor>MacLeod, Colin M</contributor><creatorcontrib>BLOUIN, J ; TEASDALE, N ; BARD, C ; FLEURY, M ; MacLeod, Colin M</creatorcontrib><description>Vision has long been considered as a single feedback system providing information about the static and the dynamic features of motor behaviors and of the environment where they take place. However, recent models for oculomanual movements have included multichanneling of visual cues ( Goodale &amp; Milner, 1992 ; Jeannerod, 1981 ; 1984 ; Paillard, 1980 , 1982 ). According to Paillard's model, a kinetic system, mostly sensitive to dynamic cues, provides directional information of the movement in the rapid distance-covering phase, and a static system, highly sensitive to position cues, provides positional signal errors. The present experiment gives kinematic evidence for the significant contribution of vision during the initial phase of rapid pointing movements when this phase is under the control of the kinetic channel. Movements having directional requirements were more accurate (directional precision) when vision of the initial portion of the trajectory was available. Times-to-peak acceleration and velocity were all shorter and their respective amplitudes were generally higher when vision was available for the first third of the trajectory than when it was not. Further, vision of the entire trajectory did not yield better precision then when vision was available for the initial phase of the movements only. Overall, the data support the existence of two corrective visual feedback systems.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1196-1961</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 0008-4255</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1878-7290</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/h0078869</identifier><identifier>PMID: 8124291</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CJEPEK</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Old Chelsea, PQ: Canadian Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Activity levels. Psychomotricity ; Adult ; Arm (Anatomy) ; Arm movements ; Attention ; Biological and medical sciences ; Control ; Distance Perception ; Eyes &amp; eyesight ; Factors ; Feedback ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Human ; Humans ; Kinesthesis ; Male ; Motor ability ; Motor Processes ; Orientation ; Perceptions ; Psychology ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Psychomotor activities ; Psychomotor Performance ; Reaction Time ; Visual feedback ; Visual Perception</subject><ispartof>Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1993-12, Vol.47 (4), p.678-696</ispartof><rights>1993 Canadian Psychological Association</rights><rights>1994 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Canadian Psychological Association Dec 1993</rights><rights>1993, Canadian Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a485t-cf6530c91a4d8439a99f41dbd36d4bfab1bcd9f3ddf1e8ec1147e6a5516be39b3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/200395598?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,21394,27924,27925,31000,33611,33612,43733</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=4085866$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8124291$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>MacLeod, Colin M</contributor><creatorcontrib>BLOUIN, J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>TEASDALE, N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>BARD, C</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>FLEURY, M</creatorcontrib><title>Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements: The Role of the Kinetic Visual Feedback System</title><title>Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology</title><addtitle>Can J Exp Psychol</addtitle><description>Vision has long been considered as a single feedback system providing information about the static and the dynamic features of motor behaviors and of the environment where they take place. However, recent models for oculomanual movements have included multichanneling of visual cues ( Goodale &amp; Milner, 1992 ; Jeannerod, 1981 ; 1984 ; Paillard, 1980 , 1982 ). According to Paillard's model, a kinetic system, mostly sensitive to dynamic cues, provides directional information of the movement in the rapid distance-covering phase, and a static system, highly sensitive to position cues, provides positional signal errors. The present experiment gives kinematic evidence for the significant contribution of vision during the initial phase of rapid pointing movements when this phase is under the control of the kinetic channel. Movements having directional requirements were more accurate (directional precision) when vision of the initial portion of the trajectory was available. Times-to-peak acceleration and velocity were all shorter and their respective amplitudes were generally higher when vision was available for the first third of the trajectory than when it was not. Further, vision of the entire trajectory did not yield better precision then when vision was available for the initial phase of the movements only. Overall, the data support the existence of two corrective visual feedback systems.</description><subject>Activity levels. Psychomotricity</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Arm (Anatomy)</subject><subject>Arm movements</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Control</subject><subject>Distance Perception</subject><subject>Eyes &amp; eyesight</subject><subject>Factors</subject><subject>Feedback</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Kinesthesis</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Motor ability</subject><subject>Motor Processes</subject><subject>Orientation</subject><subject>Perceptions</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Psychomotor activities</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Visual feedback</subject><subject>Visual Perception</subject><issn>1196-1961</issn><issn>0008-4255</issn><issn>1878-7290</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1993</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0W1r1TAYBuAgk7lNYX9AKJsMmdTlad4_jrOpg4kg-jmkecGOtumSdrB_bw7nOHHgRggJPBc3JDdCh4A_Aibi7BfGQkquXqA9kELWolF4p9xB8bpseIX2c77BuGAKu2hXQkMbBXvo9KJL3s5dHE1freI4p9hXMVTfzdS56jwN1dd45wc_zvk1ehlMn_2b7XmAfn66_LH6Ul9_-3y1Or-uDZVsrm3gjGCrwFAnKVFGqUDBtY5wR9tgWmitU4E4F8BLbwGo8NwwBrz1RLXkAJ1scqcUbxefZz102fq-N6OPS9aCN-vFnoVMEEmVIgUePYI3cUnlxVlzoISwRjRPoQZjohhTsqDj_yEoX44xV6CKer9RNsWckw96St1g0r0GrNd96T99Ffp2G7i0g3cPcFtQmb_bzk22pg_JjLbLD4xiySTnhX3YMDMZPeV7a9Lc2d5nu6RUytPWT5oKTTUX8m_oY_0v-w0SZ7Sp</recordid><startdate>19931201</startdate><enddate>19931201</enddate><creator>BLOUIN, J</creator><creator>TEASDALE, N</creator><creator>BARD, C</creator><creator>FLEURY, M</creator><general>Canadian Psychological Association</general><general>Educational Publishing Foundation</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>EOLOZ</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>ICWRT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>RYJCC</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8FQ</scope><scope>8FV</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M3G</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19931201</creationdate><title>Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements</title><author>BLOUIN, J ; TEASDALE, N ; BARD, C ; FLEURY, M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a485t-cf6530c91a4d8439a99f41dbd36d4bfab1bcd9f3ddf1e8ec1147e6a5516be39b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1993</creationdate><topic>Activity levels. Psychomotricity</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Arm (Anatomy)</topic><topic>Arm movements</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Control</topic><topic>Distance Perception</topic><topic>Eyes &amp; eyesight</topic><topic>Factors</topic><topic>Feedback</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Kinesthesis</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Motor ability</topic><topic>Motor Processes</topic><topic>Orientation</topic><topic>Perceptions</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Psychomotor activities</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Visual feedback</topic><topic>Visual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>BLOUIN, J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>TEASDALE, N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>BARD, C</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>FLEURY, M</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 01</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 28</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 41</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>ProQuest Health and Medical</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Canadian Business &amp; Current Affairs Database</collection><collection>Canadian Business &amp; Current Affairs Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>eLibrary</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>ProQuest research library</collection><collection>Science Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>CBCA Reference &amp; Current Events</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>BLOUIN, J</au><au>TEASDALE, N</au><au>BARD, C</au><au>FLEURY, M</au><au>MacLeod, Colin M</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements: The Role of the Kinetic Visual Feedback System</atitle><jtitle>Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology</jtitle><addtitle>Can J Exp Psychol</addtitle><date>1993-12-01</date><risdate>1993</risdate><volume>47</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>678</spage><epage>696</epage><pages>678-696</pages><issn>1196-1961</issn><issn>0008-4255</issn><eissn>1878-7290</eissn><coden>CJEPEK</coden><abstract>Vision has long been considered as a single feedback system providing information about the static and the dynamic features of motor behaviors and of the environment where they take place. However, recent models for oculomanual movements have included multichanneling of visual cues ( Goodale &amp; Milner, 1992 ; Jeannerod, 1981 ; 1984 ; Paillard, 1980 , 1982 ). According to Paillard's model, a kinetic system, mostly sensitive to dynamic cues, provides directional information of the movement in the rapid distance-covering phase, and a static system, highly sensitive to position cues, provides positional signal errors. The present experiment gives kinematic evidence for the significant contribution of vision during the initial phase of rapid pointing movements when this phase is under the control of the kinetic channel. Movements having directional requirements were more accurate (directional precision) when vision of the initial portion of the trajectory was available. Times-to-peak acceleration and velocity were all shorter and their respective amplitudes were generally higher when vision was available for the first third of the trajectory than when it was not. Further, vision of the entire trajectory did not yield better precision then when vision was available for the initial phase of the movements only. Overall, the data support the existence of two corrective visual feedback systems.</abstract><cop>Old Chelsea, PQ</cop><pub>Canadian Psychological Association</pub><pmid>8124291</pmid><doi>10.1037/h0078869</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1196-1961
ispartof Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1993-12, Vol.47 (4), p.678-696
issn 1196-1961
0008-4255
1878-7290
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_76262625
source APA PsycARTICLES; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Social Science Premium Collection
subjects Activity levels. Psychomotricity
Adult
Arm (Anatomy)
Arm movements
Attention
Biological and medical sciences
Control
Distance Perception
Eyes & eyesight
Factors
Feedback
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Human
Humans
Kinesthesis
Male
Motor ability
Motor Processes
Orientation
Perceptions
Psychology
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Psychomotor activities
Psychomotor Performance
Reaction Time
Visual feedback
Visual Perception
title Directional Control of Rapid Arm Movements: The Role of the Kinetic Visual Feedback System
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-28T23%3A31%3A52IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Directional%20Control%20of%20Rapid%20Arm%20Movements:%20The%20Role%20of%20the%20Kinetic%20Visual%20Feedback%20System&rft.jtitle=Canadian%20Journal%20of%20Experimental%20Psychology&rft.au=BLOUIN,%20J&rft.date=1993-12-01&rft.volume=47&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=678&rft.epage=696&rft.pages=678-696&rft.issn=1196-1961&rft.eissn=1878-7290&rft.coden=CJEPEK&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/h0078869&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E57384993%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a485t-cf6530c91a4d8439a99f41dbd36d4bfab1bcd9f3ddf1e8ec1147e6a5516be39b3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=200395598&rft_id=info:pmid/8124291&rfr_iscdi=true