Loading…
Fantasy Proneness: Hypnosis, Developmental Antecedents, and Psychopathology
This article presents a summary of the findings of our ongoing research program on the fantasy-prone person. In seven studies, nearly 6,000 college students were screened in order to obtain five samples of 156 fantasy-prone subjects. Fantasy-prone subjects (fantasizers) were selected from the upper...
Saved in:
Published in: | The American psychologist 1988-01, Vol.43 (1), p.35-44 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a379t-c7b2cf32db72e77bd07a8161a00b3c88ecf26f8a19274404e7a031a018d403f53 |
---|---|
cites | |
container_end_page | 44 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 35 |
container_title | The American psychologist |
container_volume | 43 |
creator | Lynn, Steven Jay Rhue, Judith W |
description | This article presents a summary of the findings of our ongoing research program on the fantasy-prone person. In seven studies, nearly 6,000 college students were screened in order to obtain five samples of 156 fantasy-prone subjects. Fantasy-prone subjects (fantasizers) were selected from the upper 2%-4% of the college population on a measure of imaginative involvement and contrasted with nonfantasizers (lower 2%-4%), and medium fantasy-prone subjects (middle range). General support was secured for Wilson and Barber's construct of fantasy proneness: Fantasizers were found to differ from nonfantasizers, and in many cases also from medium-range subjects, on measures of hypnotizability, imagination, waking suggestibility, hallucinatory ability, creativity, psychopathology, and childhood experiences. Differences in hypnotizability were most reliable when subjects participated in a multisession study and were screened not only with the screening inventory, but also with an interview that substantiated their fantasy-prone status. However, our findings indicated that less correspondence between fantasy proneness and hypnotizability exists than Wilson and Barber suggested. Hypnotic responsiveness is possible even in the absence of well-developed imaginative abilities, and not all fantasizers were highly hypnotizable. Fantasizers recollected being physically abused and punished to a greater degree than other subjects did and reported experiencing greater loneliness and isolation as children. Many fantasizers appeared to be relatively well-adjusted; however, a subset of fantasizers were clearly maladjusted based on self-report, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and Rorschach test data. Because of the diversity inherent in the fantasy-prone population, it is misleading to think of individuals at the extreme end of the fantasy-proneness continuum as conforming to a unitary personality type. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.35 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_78126918</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>78126918</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a379t-c7b2cf32db72e77bd07a8161a00b3c88ecf26f8a19274404e7a031a018d403f53</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kM1Lw0AQxRdRaq0ePHoQRMFb4szuZj-OUvyCgh4Uels2mw20pEncTQ79701oqSB4Gob3mzePR8gVQorA5AMAsASEWKacpZiy7IhMUbMs0RqWx2R60E_JWYzrYc2UxgmZMCq1kmJKLp9t3dm4vfkITe1rH-M5OSltFf3Ffs7I1_PT5_w1Wby_vM0fF4llUneJkzl1JaNFLqmXMi9AWoUCLUDOnFLelVSUyqKmknPgXlpgg4qq4MDKjM3I_c63Dc1372NnNqvofFXZ2jd9NFIhFRrVAN7-AddNH-ohmxHIGSgh-ADd_QchVVpzrhQOFO4oF5oYgy9NG1YbG7YGwYyNmrExMzZmODNo2Jjzeu_c5xtfHC72Ff5-tq01bdw6G7qVq3w0dtMeXH4A5Dl5cQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614308664</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Fantasy Proneness: Hypnosis, Developmental Antecedents, and Psychopathology</title><source>PsycARTICLES</source><creator>Lynn, Steven Jay ; Rhue, Judith W</creator><creatorcontrib>Lynn, Steven Jay ; Rhue, Judith W</creatorcontrib><description>This article presents a summary of the findings of our ongoing research program on the fantasy-prone person. In seven studies, nearly 6,000 college students were screened in order to obtain five samples of 156 fantasy-prone subjects. Fantasy-prone subjects (fantasizers) were selected from the upper 2%-4% of the college population on a measure of imaginative involvement and contrasted with nonfantasizers (lower 2%-4%), and medium fantasy-prone subjects (middle range). General support was secured for Wilson and Barber's construct of fantasy proneness: Fantasizers were found to differ from nonfantasizers, and in many cases also from medium-range subjects, on measures of hypnotizability, imagination, waking suggestibility, hallucinatory ability, creativity, psychopathology, and childhood experiences. Differences in hypnotizability were most reliable when subjects participated in a multisession study and were screened not only with the screening inventory, but also with an interview that substantiated their fantasy-prone status. However, our findings indicated that less correspondence between fantasy proneness and hypnotizability exists than Wilson and Barber suggested. Hypnotic responsiveness is possible even in the absence of well-developed imaginative abilities, and not all fantasizers were highly hypnotizable. Fantasizers recollected being physically abused and punished to a greater degree than other subjects did and reported experiencing greater loneliness and isolation as children. Many fantasizers appeared to be relatively well-adjusted; however, a subset of fantasizers were clearly maladjusted based on self-report, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and Rorschach test data. Because of the diversity inherent in the fantasy-prone population, it is misleading to think of individuals at the extreme end of the fantasy-proneness continuum as conforming to a unitary personality type.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0003-066X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1935-990X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.35</identifier><identifier>PMID: 3279876</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Child ; Child Abuse - psychology ; Creativity ; Early Experience ; Fantasy ; Human ; Humans ; Hypnosis ; Hypnotic Susceptibility ; Imagination ; Mental Disorders - psychology ; Psychopathology</subject><ispartof>The American psychologist, 1988-01, Vol.43 (1), p.35-44</ispartof><rights>1988 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1988, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a379t-c7b2cf32db72e77bd07a8161a00b3c88ecf26f8a19274404e7a031a018d403f53</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,4024,27923,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3279876$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Lynn, Steven Jay</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rhue, Judith W</creatorcontrib><title>Fantasy Proneness: Hypnosis, Developmental Antecedents, and Psychopathology</title><title>The American psychologist</title><addtitle>Am Psychol</addtitle><description>This article presents a summary of the findings of our ongoing research program on the fantasy-prone person. In seven studies, nearly 6,000 college students were screened in order to obtain five samples of 156 fantasy-prone subjects. Fantasy-prone subjects (fantasizers) were selected from the upper 2%-4% of the college population on a measure of imaginative involvement and contrasted with nonfantasizers (lower 2%-4%), and medium fantasy-prone subjects (middle range). General support was secured for Wilson and Barber's construct of fantasy proneness: Fantasizers were found to differ from nonfantasizers, and in many cases also from medium-range subjects, on measures of hypnotizability, imagination, waking suggestibility, hallucinatory ability, creativity, psychopathology, and childhood experiences. Differences in hypnotizability were most reliable when subjects participated in a multisession study and were screened not only with the screening inventory, but also with an interview that substantiated their fantasy-prone status. However, our findings indicated that less correspondence between fantasy proneness and hypnotizability exists than Wilson and Barber suggested. Hypnotic responsiveness is possible even in the absence of well-developed imaginative abilities, and not all fantasizers were highly hypnotizable. Fantasizers recollected being physically abused and punished to a greater degree than other subjects did and reported experiencing greater loneliness and isolation as children. Many fantasizers appeared to be relatively well-adjusted; however, a subset of fantasizers were clearly maladjusted based on self-report, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and Rorschach test data. Because of the diversity inherent in the fantasy-prone population, it is misleading to think of individuals at the extreme end of the fantasy-proneness continuum as conforming to a unitary personality type.</description><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child Abuse - psychology</subject><subject>Creativity</subject><subject>Early Experience</subject><subject>Fantasy</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hypnosis</subject><subject>Hypnotic Susceptibility</subject><subject>Imagination</subject><subject>Mental Disorders - psychology</subject><subject>Psychopathology</subject><issn>0003-066X</issn><issn>1935-990X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1988</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp1kM1Lw0AQxRdRaq0ePHoQRMFb4szuZj-OUvyCgh4Uels2mw20pEncTQ79701oqSB4Gob3mzePR8gVQorA5AMAsASEWKacpZiy7IhMUbMs0RqWx2R60E_JWYzrYc2UxgmZMCq1kmJKLp9t3dm4vfkITe1rH-M5OSltFf3Ffs7I1_PT5_w1Wby_vM0fF4llUneJkzl1JaNFLqmXMi9AWoUCLUDOnFLelVSUyqKmknPgXlpgg4qq4MDKjM3I_c63Dc1372NnNqvofFXZ2jd9NFIhFRrVAN7-AddNH-ohmxHIGSgh-ADd_QchVVpzrhQOFO4oF5oYgy9NG1YbG7YGwYyNmrExMzZmODNo2Jjzeu_c5xtfHC72Ff5-tq01bdw6G7qVq3w0dtMeXH4A5Dl5cQ</recordid><startdate>198801</startdate><enddate>198801</enddate><creator>Lynn, Steven Jay</creator><creator>Rhue, Judith W</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>EOLOZ</scope><scope>FUVTR</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>198801</creationdate><title>Fantasy Proneness</title><author>Lynn, Steven Jay ; Rhue, Judith W</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a379t-c7b2cf32db72e77bd07a8161a00b3c88ecf26f8a19274404e7a031a018d403f53</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1988</creationdate><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child Abuse - psychology</topic><topic>Creativity</topic><topic>Early Experience</topic><topic>Fantasy</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hypnosis</topic><topic>Hypnotic Susceptibility</topic><topic>Imagination</topic><topic>Mental Disorders - psychology</topic><topic>Psychopathology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Lynn, Steven Jay</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rhue, Judith W</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 01</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 06</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>PsycARTICLES (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>The American psychologist</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Lynn, Steven Jay</au><au>Rhue, Judith W</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Fantasy Proneness: Hypnosis, Developmental Antecedents, and Psychopathology</atitle><jtitle>The American psychologist</jtitle><addtitle>Am Psychol</addtitle><date>1988-01</date><risdate>1988</risdate><volume>43</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>35</spage><epage>44</epage><pages>35-44</pages><issn>0003-066X</issn><eissn>1935-990X</eissn><abstract>This article presents a summary of the findings of our ongoing research program on the fantasy-prone person. In seven studies, nearly 6,000 college students were screened in order to obtain five samples of 156 fantasy-prone subjects. Fantasy-prone subjects (fantasizers) were selected from the upper 2%-4% of the college population on a measure of imaginative involvement and contrasted with nonfantasizers (lower 2%-4%), and medium fantasy-prone subjects (middle range). General support was secured for Wilson and Barber's construct of fantasy proneness: Fantasizers were found to differ from nonfantasizers, and in many cases also from medium-range subjects, on measures of hypnotizability, imagination, waking suggestibility, hallucinatory ability, creativity, psychopathology, and childhood experiences. Differences in hypnotizability were most reliable when subjects participated in a multisession study and were screened not only with the screening inventory, but also with an interview that substantiated their fantasy-prone status. However, our findings indicated that less correspondence between fantasy proneness and hypnotizability exists than Wilson and Barber suggested. Hypnotic responsiveness is possible even in the absence of well-developed imaginative abilities, and not all fantasizers were highly hypnotizable. Fantasizers recollected being physically abused and punished to a greater degree than other subjects did and reported experiencing greater loneliness and isolation as children. Many fantasizers appeared to be relatively well-adjusted; however, a subset of fantasizers were clearly maladjusted based on self-report, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), and Rorschach test data. Because of the diversity inherent in the fantasy-prone population, it is misleading to think of individuals at the extreme end of the fantasy-proneness continuum as conforming to a unitary personality type.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><pmid>3279876</pmid><doi>10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.35</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0003-066X |
ispartof | The American psychologist, 1988-01, Vol.43 (1), p.35-44 |
issn | 0003-066X 1935-990X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_78126918 |
source | PsycARTICLES |
subjects | Child Child Abuse - psychology Creativity Early Experience Fantasy Human Humans Hypnosis Hypnotic Susceptibility Imagination Mental Disorders - psychology Psychopathology |
title | Fantasy Proneness: Hypnosis, Developmental Antecedents, and Psychopathology |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-07T16%3A18%3A20IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Fantasy%20Proneness:%20Hypnosis,%20Developmental%20Antecedents,%20and%20Psychopathology&rft.jtitle=The%20American%20psychologist&rft.au=Lynn,%20Steven%20Jay&rft.date=1988-01&rft.volume=43&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=35&rft.epage=44&rft.pages=35-44&rft.issn=0003-066X&rft.eissn=1935-990X&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.35&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E78126918%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a379t-c7b2cf32db72e77bd07a8161a00b3c88ecf26f8a19274404e7a031a018d403f53%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614308664&rft_id=info:pmid/3279876&rfr_iscdi=true |