Loading…

Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter

Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated fu...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Cognition 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168
Main Authors: Kemler Nelson, Deborah G, Frankenfield, Anne, Morris, Catherine, Blair, Elizabeth
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93
container_end_page 168
container_issue 2
container_start_page 133
container_title Cognition
container_volume 77
creator Kemler Nelson, Deborah G
Frankenfield, Anne
Morris, Catherine
Blair, Elizabeth
description Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85520763</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ615485</ericid><els_id>S0010027700000974</els_id><sourcerecordid>85520763</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUuLFTEQRoMoznj1H6gEFB-L1kq683IjMowvBlyoC1chna6eydC3MyZpQX-9ae9lFDd3VRR16iOpQ8gDBi8YMPnyMwCDBrhSzwCeA4BRTXeDHDOt2kbpVt8kx9fIEbmT82WFOq70bXLEwGjZSnFM3Le4zOfUX4RpSDg_zXTJSONIx2X2JcTZTTTMY0xbt3a0ROpdwfOYwi-kLpUwOl_yK1ouEiJdm5hy7VyhdaVguktujW7KeG9fN-Tr29MvJ--bs0_vPpy8OWu8AFYaLn3XGVCtElKjl2rwsmWcCSfAdGIcWW_UoCVTzmkw2LeCD0IPsvfY8960G_Jkl3uV4vcFc7HbkD1Ok5sxLtlqITgo2R4EFefCgD6c2GqjNK-H3JBH_4GXcUn1dNlWHQrqjzSrlNhRPsWcE472KoWtSz8tA7s6tX-c2lWYhVpXp7arew_36Uu_xeGfrZ3ECjzeAy57N43JzT7kv5xgnZG6Yvd3GKbgr6enHyUTnV5TXu_HVdKPgMlmH3D2OISEvtghhgMP_Q3XzMWi</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877056881</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><source>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</source><source>ScienceDirect Journals</source><source>ERIC</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</creator><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><description>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10986365</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGTNAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Adult ; Analysis of Variance ; Artifact categorization ; Biological and medical sciences ; Categorization ; Child ; Child development ; Child psychology ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Classification ; Cognition ; Concept Formation ; Conceptual development ; Developmental psychology ; Female ; Function Concept ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Generalization ; Generalization (Psychology) ; Humans ; Lexicology ; Male ; Mind ; Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) ; Object Naming ; Perception ; Performance Factors ; Philadelphia ; Preschool Children ; Psychological Theory ; Psychology ; Psychology, Child ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Time Factors</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168</ispartof><rights>2000 Elsevier Science B.V.</rights><rights>2000 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,31270,33224</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ615485$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=1514968$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10986365$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frankenfield, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Morris, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Artifact categorization</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Categorization</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Child psychology</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Classification</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Concept Formation</subject><subject>Conceptual development</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Function Concept</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Generalization</subject><subject>Generalization (Psychology)</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Lexicology</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind</subject><subject>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</subject><subject>Object Naming</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Performance Factors</subject><subject>Philadelphia</subject><subject>Preschool Children</subject><subject>Psychological Theory</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology, Child</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2000</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>8BJ</sourceid><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkUuLFTEQRoMoznj1H6gEFB-L1kq683IjMowvBlyoC1chna6eydC3MyZpQX-9ae9lFDd3VRR16iOpQ8gDBi8YMPnyMwCDBrhSzwCeA4BRTXeDHDOt2kbpVt8kx9fIEbmT82WFOq70bXLEwGjZSnFM3Le4zOfUX4RpSDg_zXTJSONIx2X2JcTZTTTMY0xbt3a0ROpdwfOYwi-kLpUwOl_yK1ouEiJdm5hy7VyhdaVguktujW7KeG9fN-Tr29MvJ--bs0_vPpy8OWu8AFYaLn3XGVCtElKjl2rwsmWcCSfAdGIcWW_UoCVTzmkw2LeCD0IPsvfY8960G_Jkl3uV4vcFc7HbkD1Ok5sxLtlqITgo2R4EFefCgD6c2GqjNK-H3JBH_4GXcUn1dNlWHQrqjzSrlNhRPsWcE472KoWtSz8tA7s6tX-c2lWYhVpXp7arew_36Uu_xeGfrZ3ECjzeAy57N43JzT7kv5xgnZG6Yvd3GKbgr6enHyUTnV5TXu_HVdKPgMlmH3D2OISEvtghhgMP_Q3XzMWi</recordid><startdate>20001116</startdate><enddate>20001116</enddate><creator>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creator><creator>Frankenfield, Anne</creator><creator>Morris, Catherine</creator><creator>Blair, Elizabeth</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><general>Elsevier Limited</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>JQCIK</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20001116</creationdate><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><author>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2000</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Artifact categorization</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Categorization</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Child psychology</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Classification</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Concept Formation</topic><topic>Conceptual development</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Function Concept</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Generalization</topic><topic>Generalization (Psychology)</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Lexicology</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind</topic><topic>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</topic><topic>Object Naming</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Performance Factors</topic><topic>Philadelphia</topic><topic>Preschool Children</topic><topic>Psychological Theory</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology, Child</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frankenfield, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Morris, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 33</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</au><au>Frankenfield, Anne</au><au>Morris, Catherine</au><au>Blair, Elizabeth</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ615485</ericid><atitle>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2000-11-16</date><risdate>2000</risdate><volume>77</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>133</spage><epage>168</epage><pages>133-168</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><coden>CGTNAU</coden><abstract>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>10986365</pmid><doi>10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4</doi><tpages>36</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0010-0277
ispartof Cognition, 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168
issn 0010-0277
1873-7838
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85520763
source International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS); ScienceDirect Journals; ERIC; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
subjects Adult
Analysis of Variance
Artifact categorization
Biological and medical sciences
Categorization
Child
Child development
Child psychology
Child, Preschool
Children
Classification
Cognition
Concept Formation
Conceptual development
Developmental psychology
Female
Function Concept
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Generalization
Generalization (Psychology)
Humans
Lexicology
Male
Mind
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Object Naming
Perception
Performance Factors
Philadelphia
Preschool Children
Psychological Theory
Psychology
Psychology, Child
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Time Factors
title Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-04T10%3A18%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Young%20children's%20use%20of%20functional%20information%20to%20categorize%20artifacts:%20three%20factors%20that%20matter&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Kemler%20Nelson,%20Deborah%20G&rft.date=2000-11-16&rft.volume=77&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=133&rft.epage=168&rft.pages=133-168&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.coden=CGTNAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E85520763%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877056881&rft_id=info:pmid/10986365&rft_ericid=EJ615485&rfr_iscdi=true