Loading…
Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter
Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated fu...
Saved in:
Published in: | Cognition 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93 |
container_end_page | 168 |
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 133 |
container_title | Cognition |
container_volume | 77 |
creator | Kemler Nelson, Deborah G Frankenfield, Anne Morris, Catherine Blair, Elizabeth |
description | Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85520763</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ615485</ericid><els_id>S0010027700000974</els_id><sourcerecordid>85520763</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUuLFTEQRoMoznj1H6gEFB-L1kq683IjMowvBlyoC1chna6eydC3MyZpQX-9ae9lFDd3VRR16iOpQ8gDBi8YMPnyMwCDBrhSzwCeA4BRTXeDHDOt2kbpVt8kx9fIEbmT82WFOq70bXLEwGjZSnFM3Le4zOfUX4RpSDg_zXTJSONIx2X2JcTZTTTMY0xbt3a0ROpdwfOYwi-kLpUwOl_yK1ouEiJdm5hy7VyhdaVguktujW7KeG9fN-Tr29MvJ--bs0_vPpy8OWu8AFYaLn3XGVCtElKjl2rwsmWcCSfAdGIcWW_UoCVTzmkw2LeCD0IPsvfY8960G_Jkl3uV4vcFc7HbkD1Ok5sxLtlqITgo2R4EFefCgD6c2GqjNK-H3JBH_4GXcUn1dNlWHQrqjzSrlNhRPsWcE472KoWtSz8tA7s6tX-c2lWYhVpXp7arew_36Uu_xeGfrZ3ECjzeAy57N43JzT7kv5xgnZG6Yvd3GKbgr6enHyUTnV5TXu_HVdKPgMlmH3D2OISEvtghhgMP_Q3XzMWi</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877056881</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><source>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</source><source>ScienceDirect Journals</source><source>ERIC</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</creator><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><description>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10986365</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGTNAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Adult ; Analysis of Variance ; Artifact categorization ; Biological and medical sciences ; Categorization ; Child ; Child development ; Child psychology ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Classification ; Cognition ; Concept Formation ; Conceptual development ; Developmental psychology ; Female ; Function Concept ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Generalization ; Generalization (Psychology) ; Humans ; Lexicology ; Male ; Mind ; Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) ; Object Naming ; Perception ; Performance Factors ; Philadelphia ; Preschool Children ; Psychological Theory ; Psychology ; Psychology, Child ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Time Factors</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168</ispartof><rights>2000 Elsevier Science B.V.</rights><rights>2000 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,31270,33224</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ615485$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1514968$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10986365$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frankenfield, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Morris, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Artifact categorization</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Categorization</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Child psychology</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Classification</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Concept Formation</subject><subject>Conceptual development</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Function Concept</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Generalization</subject><subject>Generalization (Psychology)</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Lexicology</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind</subject><subject>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</subject><subject>Object Naming</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Performance Factors</subject><subject>Philadelphia</subject><subject>Preschool Children</subject><subject>Psychological Theory</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology, Child</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2000</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>8BJ</sourceid><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkUuLFTEQRoMoznj1H6gEFB-L1kq683IjMowvBlyoC1chna6eydC3MyZpQX-9ae9lFDd3VRR16iOpQ8gDBi8YMPnyMwCDBrhSzwCeA4BRTXeDHDOt2kbpVt8kx9fIEbmT82WFOq70bXLEwGjZSnFM3Le4zOfUX4RpSDg_zXTJSONIx2X2JcTZTTTMY0xbt3a0ROpdwfOYwi-kLpUwOl_yK1ouEiJdm5hy7VyhdaVguktujW7KeG9fN-Tr29MvJ--bs0_vPpy8OWu8AFYaLn3XGVCtElKjl2rwsmWcCSfAdGIcWW_UoCVTzmkw2LeCD0IPsvfY8960G_Jkl3uV4vcFc7HbkD1Ok5sxLtlqITgo2R4EFefCgD6c2GqjNK-H3JBH_4GXcUn1dNlWHQrqjzSrlNhRPsWcE472KoWtSz8tA7s6tX-c2lWYhVpXp7arew_36Uu_xeGfrZ3ECjzeAy57N43JzT7kv5xgnZG6Yvd3GKbgr6enHyUTnV5TXu_HVdKPgMlmH3D2OISEvtghhgMP_Q3XzMWi</recordid><startdate>20001116</startdate><enddate>20001116</enddate><creator>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creator><creator>Frankenfield, Anne</creator><creator>Morris, Catherine</creator><creator>Blair, Elizabeth</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><general>Elsevier Limited</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>JQCIK</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20001116</creationdate><title>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</title><author>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G ; Frankenfield, Anne ; Morris, Catherine ; Blair, Elizabeth</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2000</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Artifact categorization</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Categorization</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Child psychology</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Classification</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Concept Formation</topic><topic>Conceptual development</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Function Concept</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Generalization</topic><topic>Generalization (Psychology)</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Lexicology</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind</topic><topic>Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)</topic><topic>Object Naming</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Performance Factors</topic><topic>Philadelphia</topic><topic>Preschool Children</topic><topic>Psychological Theory</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology, Child</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frankenfield, Anne</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Morris, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Blair, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 33</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kemler Nelson, Deborah G</au><au>Frankenfield, Anne</au><au>Morris, Catherine</au><au>Blair, Elizabeth</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ615485</ericid><atitle>Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2000-11-16</date><risdate>2000</risdate><volume>77</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>133</spage><epage>168</epage><pages>133-168</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><coden>CGTNAU</coden><abstract>Three experiments addressed factors that might influence whether or not young children take into account function, as opposed to overall appearance or shape, when they extend the names of novel artifacts. Experiment 1 showed that 4-year-olds more often extend a name on the basis of a demonstrated function when that function provides a plausible causal account of perceptible object structure. Experiment 2 showed that they more often extend a name by function when they respond slowly, and hence thoughtfully. Experiment 3 demonstrated that they are more likely to take function into account when they extend names than when they judge similarities. Comparisons of lexical and non-lexical conditions in younger children failed to show any differences. Overall, the findings suggest that by 4 years of age, children may learn names as labels for novel artifact kinds rather than perceptual classes, and that the processes by which they categorize may be mindful and reflective, as in adults.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>10986365</pmid><doi>10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4</doi><tpages>36</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0010-0277 |
ispartof | Cognition, 2000-11, Vol.77 (2), p.133-168 |
issn | 0010-0277 1873-7838 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85520763 |
source | International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS); ScienceDirect Journals; ERIC; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
subjects | Adult Analysis of Variance Artifact categorization Biological and medical sciences Categorization Child Child development Child psychology Child, Preschool Children Classification Cognition Concept Formation Conceptual development Developmental psychology Female Function Concept Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Generalization Generalization (Psychology) Humans Lexicology Male Mind Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) Object Naming Perception Performance Factors Philadelphia Preschool Children Psychological Theory Psychology Psychology, Child Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Time Factors |
title | Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-04T10%3A18%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Young%20children's%20use%20of%20functional%20information%20to%20categorize%20artifacts:%20three%20factors%20that%20matter&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Kemler%20Nelson,%20Deborah%20G&rft.date=2000-11-16&rft.volume=77&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=133&rft.epage=168&rft.pages=133-168&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.coden=CGTNAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00097-4&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E85520763%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-26c4490737568ec67dc631215a50945ff1b97d8617aa809eb352d58d6bceb2b93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877056881&rft_id=info:pmid/10986365&rft_ericid=EJ615485&rfr_iscdi=true |