Loading…
Test–retest bias, reliability, and regression equations for neuropsychological measures repeated over a 12–16-week period
The interpretation of neurobehavioral change over time requires knowledge of the test–retest characteristics of the measures. Without this information it is not possible to distinguish a true change (i.e., one reflecting the occurrence or resolution of an intervening process) from that occurring on...
Saved in:
Published in: | Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2001-07, Vol.7 (5), p.597-605 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
Summary: | The interpretation of neurobehavioral change over time
requires knowledge of the test–retest characteristics
of the measures. Without this information it is not possible
to distinguish a true change (i.e., one reflecting the
occurrence or resolution of an intervening process) from
that occurring on the basis of chance or systematic bias.
We tested a group of 72 healthy young to middle aged adults
twice over a 12-to-16-week interval in order to observe
the change in scores over time when there was no known
intervention. The test battery consisted of seven commonly
used cognitive measures and the Profile of Mood States
(POMS). Test–retest regression equations were calculated
for each measure using initial performance, age, education,
and a measure of general intellectual function (Wonderlic
Personnel Test) as regressors. Test–retest correlations
ranged from .39 (POMS Fatigue) to .89 (Digit Symbol). Cognitive
measures generally yielded higher correlations than did
the POMS. Univariate regressions based only on initial
performance adequately predicted retest performance for
the majority of measures. Age and education had a relatively
minor influence. Practice effects and regression to the
mean were common. These test–retest regression equations
can be used to predict retest scores when there has been
no known intervention. They can also be used to generate
statistical statements regarding the significance of change
in an individual's performance over a 12-to-16-week
interval. (JINS, 2001, 7, 597–605.) |
---|---|
ISSN: | 1355-6177 1469-7661 |
DOI: | 10.1017/S1355617701755075 |