Loading…

Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements

1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal)...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:The Journal of physiology 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182
Main Authors: Collewijn, H, Erkelens, C J, Steinman, R M
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063
cites
container_end_page 182
container_issue 1
container_start_page 157
container_title The Journal of physiology
container_volume 404
creator Collewijn, H
Erkelens, C J
Steinman, R M
description 1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3. Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades. 7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version
doi_str_mv 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1190820</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>78767450</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkEtv1DAUhS0EKkPhJ4CyQLDK4Ff82CAxFbSgSrAoa8tjO40rJx7spFX49TjKdAQ7Vtb1-e45VweANwhuEULkw92hm7OPYYukENt8gIhjQZ-ADaJM1pxL8hRsIMS4JrxBz8GLnO8gRARKeQbOCG4IxXIDdjs_RDMFnSoT65isH_To41DFtuqmXg9VF5P_HYdRhyprY7T1pnKzq_p473o3jPkleNbqkN2r43sOfn75fHNxVV9_v_x68em6Ng2CvN4T6hopIKQWOmw4kYbtDbWWl7nFlGshuWWyXLi32lihLSOOtkgbTFvIyDn4uPoepn3vrCnZSQd1SL7XaVZRe_WvMvhO3cZ7hZCEAsNi8O5okOKvyeVR9T4bF4IeXJyy4oIzTpsFZCtoUsw5ufYUgqBa2leP7aulffXYfll8_feJp7Vj3UV_e9R1Njq0SQ_G5xPGERGM4YLtVuzBBzf_Z7i6-fZj-aCQItTwYvJ-Nen8bffgk1PrWo7Gu3FWhVNILeQfcUm1SA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>78767450</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</creator><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><description>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3. Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades. 7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-3751</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-7793</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284</identifier><identifier>PMID: 3253429</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JPHYA7</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford: The Physiological Society</publisher><subject>Adult ; Biological and medical sciences ; Convergence, Ocular ; Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision ; Eye Movements ; Fixation, Ocular ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Saccades ; Time Factors ; Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs ; Vision, Binocular</subject><ispartof>The Journal of physiology, 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182</ispartof><rights>1988 The Physiological Society</rights><rights>1989 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1190820/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1190820/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,27924,27925,53791,53793</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=7138662$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3253429$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Erkelens, C J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><title>The Journal of physiology</title><addtitle>J Physiol</addtitle><description>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3. Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades. 7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Convergence, Ocular</subject><subject>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</subject><subject>Eye Movements</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Saccades</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><subject>Vision, Binocular</subject><issn>0022-3751</issn><issn>1469-7793</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1988</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqNkEtv1DAUhS0EKkPhJ4CyQLDK4Ff82CAxFbSgSrAoa8tjO40rJx7spFX49TjKdAQ7Vtb1-e45VweANwhuEULkw92hm7OPYYukENt8gIhjQZ-ADaJM1pxL8hRsIMS4JrxBz8GLnO8gRARKeQbOCG4IxXIDdjs_RDMFnSoT65isH_To41DFtuqmXg9VF5P_HYdRhyprY7T1pnKzq_p473o3jPkleNbqkN2r43sOfn75fHNxVV9_v_x68em6Ng2CvN4T6hopIKQWOmw4kYbtDbWWl7nFlGshuWWyXLi32lihLSOOtkgbTFvIyDn4uPoepn3vrCnZSQd1SL7XaVZRe_WvMvhO3cZ7hZCEAsNi8O5okOKvyeVR9T4bF4IeXJyy4oIzTpsFZCtoUsw5ufYUgqBa2leP7aulffXYfll8_feJp7Vj3UV_e9R1Njq0SQ_G5xPGERGM4YLtVuzBBzf_Z7i6-fZj-aCQItTwYvJ-Nen8bffgk1PrWo7Gu3FWhVNILeQfcUm1SA</recordid><startdate>19881001</startdate><enddate>19881001</enddate><creator>Collewijn, H</creator><creator>Erkelens, C J</creator><creator>Steinman, R M</creator><general>The Physiological Society</general><general>Blackwell</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19881001</creationdate><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><author>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1988</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Convergence, Ocular</topic><topic>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</topic><topic>Eye Movements</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Saccades</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</topic><topic>Vision, Binocular</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Erkelens, C J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>The Journal of physiology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Collewijn, H</au><au>Erkelens, C J</au><au>Steinman, R M</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</atitle><jtitle>The Journal of physiology</jtitle><addtitle>J Physiol</addtitle><date>1988-10-01</date><risdate>1988</risdate><volume>404</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>157</spage><epage>182</epage><pages>157-182</pages><issn>0022-3751</issn><eissn>1469-7793</eissn><coden>JPHYA7</coden><abstract>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3. Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades. 7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</abstract><cop>Oxford</cop><pub>The Physiological Society</pub><pmid>3253429</pmid><doi>10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284</doi><tpages>26</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0022-3751
ispartof The Journal of physiology, 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182
issn 0022-3751
1469-7793
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1190820
source PubMed Central
subjects Adult
Biological and medical sciences
Convergence, Ocular
Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision
Eye Movements
Fixation, Ocular
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Male
Middle Aged
Saccades
Time Factors
Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs
Vision, Binocular
title Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T14%3A25%3A07IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Binocular%20co-ordination%20of%20human%20horizontal%20saccadic%20eye%20movements&rft.jtitle=The%20Journal%20of%20physiology&rft.au=Collewijn,%20H&rft.date=1988-10-01&rft.volume=404&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=157&rft.epage=182&rft.pages=157-182&rft.issn=0022-3751&rft.eissn=1469-7793&rft.coden=JPHYA7&rft_id=info:doi/10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E78767450%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=78767450&rft_id=info:pmid/3253429&rfr_iscdi=true