Loading…
Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements
1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal)...
Saved in:
Published in: | The Journal of physiology 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063 |
---|---|
cites | |
container_end_page | 182 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 157 |
container_title | The Journal of physiology |
container_volume | 404 |
creator | Collewijn, H Erkelens, C J Steinman, R M |
description | 1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor
range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction
vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary
positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation
on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible
targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3.
Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the
target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting
by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated
at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related
to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness
values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to
about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well
yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration
and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently
by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities
of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades.
7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject
variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence
component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version |
doi_str_mv | 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1190820</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>78767450</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkEtv1DAUhS0EKkPhJ4CyQLDK4Ff82CAxFbSgSrAoa8tjO40rJx7spFX49TjKdAQ7Vtb1-e45VweANwhuEULkw92hm7OPYYukENt8gIhjQZ-ADaJM1pxL8hRsIMS4JrxBz8GLnO8gRARKeQbOCG4IxXIDdjs_RDMFnSoT65isH_To41DFtuqmXg9VF5P_HYdRhyprY7T1pnKzq_p473o3jPkleNbqkN2r43sOfn75fHNxVV9_v_x68em6Ng2CvN4T6hopIKQWOmw4kYbtDbWWl7nFlGshuWWyXLi32lihLSOOtkgbTFvIyDn4uPoepn3vrCnZSQd1SL7XaVZRe_WvMvhO3cZ7hZCEAsNi8O5okOKvyeVR9T4bF4IeXJyy4oIzTpsFZCtoUsw5ufYUgqBa2leP7aulffXYfll8_feJp7Vj3UV_e9R1Njq0SQ_G5xPGERGM4YLtVuzBBzf_Z7i6-fZj-aCQItTwYvJ-Nen8bffgk1PrWo7Gu3FWhVNILeQfcUm1SA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>78767450</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</creator><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><description>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor
range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction
vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary
positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation
on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible
targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3.
Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the
target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting
by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated
at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related
to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness
values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to
about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well
yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration
and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently
by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities
of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades.
7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject
variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence
component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally
smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-3751</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-7793</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284</identifier><identifier>PMID: 3253429</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JPHYA7</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford: The Physiological Society</publisher><subject>Adult ; Biological and medical sciences ; Convergence, Ocular ; Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision ; Eye Movements ; Fixation, Ocular ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Saccades ; Time Factors ; Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs ; Vision, Binocular</subject><ispartof>The Journal of physiology, 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182</ispartof><rights>1988 The Physiological Society</rights><rights>1989 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1190820/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1190820/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,885,27924,27925,53791,53793</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=7138662$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3253429$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Erkelens, C J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><title>The Journal of physiology</title><addtitle>J Physiol</addtitle><description>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor
range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction
vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary
positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation
on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible
targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3.
Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the
target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting
by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated
at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related
to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness
values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to
about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well
yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration
and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently
by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities
of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades.
7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject
variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence
component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally
smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Convergence, Ocular</subject><subject>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</subject><subject>Eye Movements</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Saccades</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><subject>Vision, Binocular</subject><issn>0022-3751</issn><issn>1469-7793</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1988</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqNkEtv1DAUhS0EKkPhJ4CyQLDK4Ff82CAxFbSgSrAoa8tjO40rJx7spFX49TjKdAQ7Vtb1-e45VweANwhuEULkw92hm7OPYYukENt8gIhjQZ-ADaJM1pxL8hRsIMS4JrxBz8GLnO8gRARKeQbOCG4IxXIDdjs_RDMFnSoT65isH_To41DFtuqmXg9VF5P_HYdRhyprY7T1pnKzq_p473o3jPkleNbqkN2r43sOfn75fHNxVV9_v_x68em6Ng2CvN4T6hopIKQWOmw4kYbtDbWWl7nFlGshuWWyXLi32lihLSOOtkgbTFvIyDn4uPoepn3vrCnZSQd1SL7XaVZRe_WvMvhO3cZ7hZCEAsNi8O5okOKvyeVR9T4bF4IeXJyy4oIzTpsFZCtoUsw5ufYUgqBa2leP7aulffXYfll8_feJp7Vj3UV_e9R1Njq0SQ_G5xPGERGM4YLtVuzBBzf_Z7i6-fZj-aCQItTwYvJ-Nen8bffgk1PrWo7Gu3FWhVNILeQfcUm1SA</recordid><startdate>19881001</startdate><enddate>19881001</enddate><creator>Collewijn, H</creator><creator>Erkelens, C J</creator><creator>Steinman, R M</creator><general>The Physiological Society</general><general>Blackwell</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19881001</creationdate><title>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</title><author>Collewijn, H ; Erkelens, C J ; Steinman, R M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1988</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Convergence, Ocular</topic><topic>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</topic><topic>Eye Movements</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Saccades</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</topic><topic>Vision, Binocular</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Collewijn, H</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Erkelens, C J</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinman, R M</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>The Journal of physiology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Collewijn, H</au><au>Erkelens, C J</au><au>Steinman, R M</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements</atitle><jtitle>The Journal of physiology</jtitle><addtitle>J Physiol</addtitle><date>1988-10-01</date><risdate>1988</risdate><volume>404</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>157</spage><epage>182</epage><pages>157-182</pages><issn>0022-3751</issn><eissn>1469-7793</eissn><coden>JPHYA7</coden><abstract>1. The binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccades was analysed for the first time systematically over the full oculomotor
range with a precise and accurate scleral sensor coil technique. Effects of amplitude (1.25-80 deg), direction (adduction
vs. abduction and centrifugal vs. centripetal) and eccentricity (symmetrical about primary or between primary and secondary
positions) were systematically investigated in three subjects). 2. To minimize extraneous effects of stimulus presentation
on the programming of saccades, subjects were instructed to voluntarily change their gaze between two continuously visible
targets. These were positioned on an iso-vergence locus, and thus contained no stimulus for disjunctive eye movements. 3.
Under these conditions the amplitudes of the primary saccades of the two eyes were remarkably accurate; undershooting of the
target by about 0.5 deg (independent of amplitude in the range 10-70 deg) was typical. This finding contrasts with the undershooting
by about 10% described in the literature as characteristic for other stimulus conditions. 4. Saccadic peak velocities saturated
at a mean asymptotic level of 502 +/- 32 (S.D.) deg/s for saccades of 40 deg and larger. The duration was linearly related
to amplitude for saccades up to 50 deg; for saccades of larger sizes the duration increased progressively more steeply. Skewness
values (acceleration time as a fraction of total saccadic duration) decreased from about 0.45 for saccades up to 10 deg to
about 0.20 for saccades of 50 deg and larger. 5. Binocular saccades showed an abduction-adduction asymmetry and were not well
yoked dynamically. The saccades of the abducting eye consistently had a larger size, a higher peak velocity, a shorter duration
and were more skewed than the concomitant adducting saccades of the fellow eye. As a result, the eyes diverged transiently
by as much as 3 deg during horizontal saccades. 6. Saccades also showed a marked centrifugal-centripetal asymmetry. Peak velocities
of saccades towards the primary position were about 10% higher than peak velocities of corresponding centrifugal saccades.
7. These directional asymmetries were the main source of variability in the pool of saccades. In comparison, intra- and intersubject
variability was minor in our sample. 8. Post-saccadic drift consisted of a vergence and a version component. The vergence
component of this drift was a continuation of the vergence movement occurring during saccades. The version component, generally
smaller than the vergence component, was directed towards the target position.</abstract><cop>Oxford</cop><pub>The Physiological Society</pub><pmid>3253429</pmid><doi>10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284</doi><tpages>26</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0022-3751 |
ispartof | The Journal of physiology, 1988-10, Vol.404 (1), p.157-182 |
issn | 0022-3751 1469-7793 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1190820 |
source | PubMed Central |
subjects | Adult Biological and medical sciences Convergence, Ocular Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision Eye Movements Fixation, Ocular Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Male Middle Aged Saccades Time Factors Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs Vision, Binocular |
title | Binocular co-ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T14%3A25%3A07IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Binocular%20co-ordination%20of%20human%20horizontal%20saccadic%20eye%20movements&rft.jtitle=The%20Journal%20of%20physiology&rft.au=Collewijn,%20H&rft.date=1988-10-01&rft.volume=404&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=157&rft.epage=182&rft.pages=157-182&rft.issn=0022-3751&rft.eissn=1469-7793&rft.coden=JPHYA7&rft_id=info:doi/10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017284&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E78767450%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5107-b34e598004d0e2c739c6bc4dd7d0ef247a897d69309bdacd8ad63e4f1ac24f063%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=78767450&rft_id=info:pmid/3253429&rfr_iscdi=true |