Loading…

RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS

Reinforcing an alternative response in the same context as a target response reduces the rate of occurrence but increases the persistence of that target response. Applied researchers who use such techniques to decrease the rate of a target problem behavior risk inadvertently increasing the persisten...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior 2012-09, Vol.98 (2), p.169-189
Main Authors: Podlesnik, Christopher A., Bai, John Y. H., Elliffe, Douglas
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533
container_end_page 189
container_issue 2
container_start_page 169
container_title Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior
container_volume 98
creator Podlesnik, Christopher A.
Bai, John Y. H.
Elliffe, Douglas
description Reinforcing an alternative response in the same context as a target response reduces the rate of occurrence but increases the persistence of that target response. Applied researchers who use such techniques to decrease the rate of a target problem behavior risk inadvertently increasing the persistence of the same problem behavior. Behavioral momentum theory asserts that the increased persistence is a function of the alternative reinforcement enhancing the Pavlovian relation between the target stimulus context and reinforcement. A method showing promise for reducing the persistence‐enhancing effects of alternative reinforcement is to train the alternative response in a separate stimulus context before combining with the target stimulus in extinction. The present study replicated previous findings using pigeons by showing that combining an “alternative” richer VI schedule (96 reinforcers/ hr) with a “target” leaner VI schedule (24 reinforcers/hr) reduced resistance to extinction of target responding compared with concurrent training of the alternative and target responses (totaling 120 reinforcers/hr). We also found less relapse with a reinstatement procedure following extinction with separate‐context training, supporting previous findings that training conditions similarly influence both resistance to extinction and relapse. Finally, combining the alternative stimulus context was less disruptive to target responding previously trained in the concurrent schedule, relative to combining with the target response trained alone. Overall, the present findings suggest the technique of combining stimulus contexts associated with alternative responses with those associated with target responses disrupts target responding. Furthermore, the effectiveness of this disruption is a function of training context of reinforcement for target responding, consistent with assertions of behavioral momentum theory.
doi_str_mv 10.1901/jeab.2012.98-169
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3449854</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ981775</ericid><sourcerecordid>1113215420</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkc1v0zAYxi0EYmVw54BQJC5cUvzacWJfkNIsjFRtCksKExfLcRyWkjYjaYH99zjLqIDLDv6Qn9_z2n4fhJ4DnoLA8GZjVDElGMhUcBd88QBNQFDu0gDgIZpgTIjL7HyCnvT9xm6EH5DH6IRQjDkjMEGzizhLsjxMo9jJV058mSdplCer1AnTM-ciXoQfsthJUidaLWdJGp85WZ4s14t1Zk_S3PLZU_SoUk1vnt2tp2j9Ls6j9-5idZ5E4cLVzKfUpbwUVeUzTxcF11VgCqUF9xjFvmbUI9oEYEhlR4kVgbIoNcdYgfG1MAWj9BS9HeteH4qtKbXZ7TvVyOuu3qruRraqlv8qu_pKfm1_SOp5gjPPFnh9V6Brvx9Mv5fbutemadTOtIdeAgAlwDyC70cxxz54VAzoq__QTXvodrYTAwWc-OSWwiOlu7bvO1Md3w1YDlnKIUs5ZCkFlzZLa3n593-Phj_hWeDFCJiu1kc5ngsOQcCs7I_yz7oxN_feJ-dxOPPgttHuaKz7vfl1NKrum_QDGjD5OT2Xy9mXfP7pI5OX9Dfa8b17</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1081826290</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS</title><source>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</source><source>Wiley-Blackwell Read &amp; Publish Collection</source><source>ERIC</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Education Collection</source><creator>Podlesnik, Christopher A. ; Bai, John Y. H. ; Elliffe, Douglas</creator><creatorcontrib>Podlesnik, Christopher A. ; Bai, John Y. H. ; Elliffe, Douglas</creatorcontrib><description>Reinforcing an alternative response in the same context as a target response reduces the rate of occurrence but increases the persistence of that target response. Applied researchers who use such techniques to decrease the rate of a target problem behavior risk inadvertently increasing the persistence of the same problem behavior. Behavioral momentum theory asserts that the increased persistence is a function of the alternative reinforcement enhancing the Pavlovian relation between the target stimulus context and reinforcement. A method showing promise for reducing the persistence‐enhancing effects of alternative reinforcement is to train the alternative response in a separate stimulus context before combining with the target stimulus in extinction. The present study replicated previous findings using pigeons by showing that combining an “alternative” richer VI schedule (96 reinforcers/ hr) with a “target” leaner VI schedule (24 reinforcers/hr) reduced resistance to extinction of target responding compared with concurrent training of the alternative and target responses (totaling 120 reinforcers/hr). We also found less relapse with a reinstatement procedure following extinction with separate‐context training, supporting previous findings that training conditions similarly influence both resistance to extinction and relapse. Finally, combining the alternative stimulus context was less disruptive to target responding previously trained in the concurrent schedule, relative to combining with the target response trained alone. Overall, the present findings suggest the technique of combining stimulus contexts associated with alternative responses with those associated with target responses disrupts target responding. Furthermore, the effectiveness of this disruption is a function of training context of reinforcement for target responding, consistent with assertions of behavioral momentum theory.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-5002</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1938-3711</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2012.98-169</identifier><identifier>PMID: 23008521</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JEABAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>Animal Behavior ; Animals ; Behavior ; Behavior Modification ; Behavior Problems ; behavioral momentum theory ; Behavioral Science Research ; Classical Conditioning ; Columbidae ; compound stimuli ; concurrent schedules ; Conditioning, Operant ; Experiments ; extinction ; Extinction, Psychological ; Feedback (Response) ; Food ; key peck ; Persistence ; pigeon ; Reinforcement ; Reinforcement Schedule ; Reinforcement, Psychology ; Resistance to Change ; Stimuli ; Teenagers</subject><ispartof>Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 2012-09, Vol.98 (2), p.169-189</ispartof><rights>2012 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</rights><rights>Copyright Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Sep 2012</rights><rights>Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Inc. 2012</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1081826290/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1081826290?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,881,21357,21373,27901,27902,33588,33589,33854,33855,43709,43856,53766,53768,74192,74367</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ981775$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23008521$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Podlesnik, Christopher A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bai, John Y. H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Elliffe, Douglas</creatorcontrib><title>RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS</title><title>Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior</title><addtitle>J Exp Anal Behav</addtitle><description>Reinforcing an alternative response in the same context as a target response reduces the rate of occurrence but increases the persistence of that target response. Applied researchers who use such techniques to decrease the rate of a target problem behavior risk inadvertently increasing the persistence of the same problem behavior. Behavioral momentum theory asserts that the increased persistence is a function of the alternative reinforcement enhancing the Pavlovian relation between the target stimulus context and reinforcement. A method showing promise for reducing the persistence‐enhancing effects of alternative reinforcement is to train the alternative response in a separate stimulus context before combining with the target stimulus in extinction. The present study replicated previous findings using pigeons by showing that combining an “alternative” richer VI schedule (96 reinforcers/ hr) with a “target” leaner VI schedule (24 reinforcers/hr) reduced resistance to extinction of target responding compared with concurrent training of the alternative and target responses (totaling 120 reinforcers/hr). We also found less relapse with a reinstatement procedure following extinction with separate‐context training, supporting previous findings that training conditions similarly influence both resistance to extinction and relapse. Finally, combining the alternative stimulus context was less disruptive to target responding previously trained in the concurrent schedule, relative to combining with the target response trained alone. Overall, the present findings suggest the technique of combining stimulus contexts associated with alternative responses with those associated with target responses disrupts target responding. Furthermore, the effectiveness of this disruption is a function of training context of reinforcement for target responding, consistent with assertions of behavioral momentum theory.</description><subject>Animal Behavior</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Behavior Modification</subject><subject>Behavior Problems</subject><subject>behavioral momentum theory</subject><subject>Behavioral Science Research</subject><subject>Classical Conditioning</subject><subject>Columbidae</subject><subject>compound stimuli</subject><subject>concurrent schedules</subject><subject>Conditioning, Operant</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>extinction</subject><subject>Extinction, Psychological</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Food</subject><subject>key peck</subject><subject>Persistence</subject><subject>pigeon</subject><subject>Reinforcement</subject><subject>Reinforcement Schedule</subject><subject>Reinforcement, Psychology</subject><subject>Resistance to Change</subject><subject>Stimuli</subject><subject>Teenagers</subject><issn>0022-5002</issn><issn>1938-3711</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CJNVE</sourceid><sourceid>M0P</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkc1v0zAYxi0EYmVw54BQJC5cUvzacWJfkNIsjFRtCksKExfLcRyWkjYjaYH99zjLqIDLDv6Qn9_z2n4fhJ4DnoLA8GZjVDElGMhUcBd88QBNQFDu0gDgIZpgTIjL7HyCnvT9xm6EH5DH6IRQjDkjMEGzizhLsjxMo9jJV058mSdplCer1AnTM-ciXoQfsthJUidaLWdJGp85WZ4s14t1Zk_S3PLZU_SoUk1vnt2tp2j9Ls6j9-5idZ5E4cLVzKfUpbwUVeUzTxcF11VgCqUF9xjFvmbUI9oEYEhlR4kVgbIoNcdYgfG1MAWj9BS9HeteH4qtKbXZ7TvVyOuu3qruRraqlv8qu_pKfm1_SOp5gjPPFnh9V6Brvx9Mv5fbutemadTOtIdeAgAlwDyC70cxxz54VAzoq__QTXvodrYTAwWc-OSWwiOlu7bvO1Md3w1YDlnKIUs5ZCkFlzZLa3n593-Phj_hWeDFCJiu1kc5ngsOQcCs7I_yz7oxN_feJ-dxOPPgttHuaKz7vfl1NKrum_QDGjD5OT2Xy9mXfP7pI5OX9Dfa8b17</recordid><startdate>201209</startdate><enddate>201209</enddate><creator>Podlesnik, Christopher A.</creator><creator>Bai, John Y. H.</creator><creator>Elliffe, Douglas</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><general>Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</general><general>Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Inc</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88A</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PHGZM</scope><scope>PHGZT</scope><scope>PJZUB</scope><scope>PKEHL</scope><scope>POGQB</scope><scope>PPXIY</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQGLB</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PRQQA</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201209</creationdate><title>RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS</title><author>Podlesnik, Christopher A. ; Bai, John Y. H. ; Elliffe, Douglas</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><topic>Animal Behavior</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Behavior Modification</topic><topic>Behavior Problems</topic><topic>behavioral momentum theory</topic><topic>Behavioral Science Research</topic><topic>Classical Conditioning</topic><topic>Columbidae</topic><topic>compound stimuli</topic><topic>concurrent schedules</topic><topic>Conditioning, Operant</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>extinction</topic><topic>Extinction, Psychological</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Food</topic><topic>key peck</topic><topic>Persistence</topic><topic>pigeon</topic><topic>Reinforcement</topic><topic>Reinforcement Schedule</topic><topic>Reinforcement, Psychology</topic><topic>Resistance to Change</topic><topic>Stimuli</topic><topic>Teenagers</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Podlesnik, Christopher A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bai, John Y. H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Elliffe, Douglas</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest_Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Biology Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Biological Sciences</collection><collection>Education Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>PML(ProQuest Medical Library)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest research library</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Research Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)</collection><collection>ProQuest Sociology &amp; Social Sciences Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Health &amp; Nursing</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Applied &amp; Life Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Podlesnik, Christopher A.</au><au>Bai, John Y. H.</au><au>Elliffe, Douglas</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ981775</ericid><atitle>RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS</atitle><jtitle>Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior</jtitle><addtitle>J Exp Anal Behav</addtitle><date>2012-09</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>98</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>169</spage><epage>189</epage><pages>169-189</pages><issn>0022-5002</issn><eissn>1938-3711</eissn><coden>JEABAU</coden><abstract>Reinforcing an alternative response in the same context as a target response reduces the rate of occurrence but increases the persistence of that target response. Applied researchers who use such techniques to decrease the rate of a target problem behavior risk inadvertently increasing the persistence of the same problem behavior. Behavioral momentum theory asserts that the increased persistence is a function of the alternative reinforcement enhancing the Pavlovian relation between the target stimulus context and reinforcement. A method showing promise for reducing the persistence‐enhancing effects of alternative reinforcement is to train the alternative response in a separate stimulus context before combining with the target stimulus in extinction. The present study replicated previous findings using pigeons by showing that combining an “alternative” richer VI schedule (96 reinforcers/ hr) with a “target” leaner VI schedule (24 reinforcers/hr) reduced resistance to extinction of target responding compared with concurrent training of the alternative and target responses (totaling 120 reinforcers/hr). We also found less relapse with a reinstatement procedure following extinction with separate‐context training, supporting previous findings that training conditions similarly influence both resistance to extinction and relapse. Finally, combining the alternative stimulus context was less disruptive to target responding previously trained in the concurrent schedule, relative to combining with the target response trained alone. Overall, the present findings suggest the technique of combining stimulus contexts associated with alternative responses with those associated with target responses disrupts target responding. Furthermore, the effectiveness of this disruption is a function of training context of reinforcement for target responding, consistent with assertions of behavioral momentum theory.</abstract><cop>Oxford, UK</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><pmid>23008521</pmid><doi>10.1901/jeab.2012.98-169</doi><tpages>21</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0022-5002
ispartof Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 2012-09, Vol.98 (2), p.169-189
issn 0022-5002
1938-3711
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3449854
source Social Science Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3); Wiley-Blackwell Read & Publish Collection; ERIC; PubMed Central; Education Collection
subjects Animal Behavior
Animals
Behavior
Behavior Modification
Behavior Problems
behavioral momentum theory
Behavioral Science Research
Classical Conditioning
Columbidae
compound stimuli
concurrent schedules
Conditioning, Operant
Experiments
extinction
Extinction, Psychological
Feedback (Response)
Food
key peck
Persistence
pigeon
Reinforcement
Reinforcement Schedule
Reinforcement, Psychology
Resistance to Change
Stimuli
Teenagers
title RESISTANCE TO EXTINCTION AND RELAPSE IN COMBINED STIMULUS CONTEXTS
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-22T11%3A57%3A57IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=RESISTANCE%20TO%20EXTINCTION%20AND%20RELAPSE%20IN%20COMBINED%20STIMULUS%20CONTEXTS&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20the%20experimental%20analysis%20of%20behavior&rft.au=Podlesnik,%20Christopher%20A.&rft.date=2012-09&rft.volume=98&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=169&rft.epage=189&rft.pages=169-189&rft.issn=0022-5002&rft.eissn=1938-3711&rft.coden=JEABAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1901/jeab.2012.98-169&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1113215420%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5633-38d9ff654cbb8cf7ebac9845306c5342ce71e2f1e2d0a21dbdc800a1e6c9eb533%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1081826290&rft_id=info:pmid/23008521&rft_ericid=EJ981775&rfr_iscdi=true