Loading…
Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence
Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations...
Saved in:
Published in: | Intelligence (Norwood) 2015-01, Vol.48, p.30-36 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013 |
container_end_page | 36 |
container_issue | |
container_start_page | 30 |
container_title | Intelligence (Norwood) |
container_volume | 48 |
creator | von Stumm, Sophie Plomin, Robert |
description | Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations between SES and individual differences in the intelligence starting point (intercept) and in the rate and direction of change in scores (slope and quadratic term) from infancy through adolescence in 14,853 children from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), assessed 9 times on IQ between the ages of 2 and 16years. SES was significantly associated with intelligence growth factors: higher SES was related both to a higher starting point in infancy and to greater gains in intelligence over time. Specifically, children from low SES families scored on average 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES backgrounds; by age 16, this difference had almost tripled. Although these key results did not vary across girls and boys, we observed gender differences in the development of intelligence in early childhood. Overall, SES was shown to be associated with individual differences in intercepts as well as slopes of intelligence. However, this finding does not warrant causal interpretations of the relationship between SES and the development of intelligence.
•IQ growth trajectories were modeled in British children from age 2 to 16years.•Children's socioeconomic background (SES) was associated with IQ growth.•High and low SES children differed by 6 IQ points at age 2.•By age 16, this IQ difference between high and low SES children had tripled. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4641149</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0160289614001433</els_id><sourcerecordid>1660415907</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkUtr3DAUhUVoaaZJ_kEohm668eReWZblTaGEtikEsmi6Fhr5ekaDbaWSnJJ_H5lJ08eidCW4-u7jnMPYOcIaAeXFfu2mRMOw5oAil9YA_IitUDVV2bRV_YKtMgYlV608Zq9j3ANAjUK9YsdcSgEVyBW7_eqt82T95Edni5hMmmNhpq5IOyq2wf9Iu8L3xWGX29JkqeiDH3OlN5N9yFzw83ZXmM4PFO0CnLKXvRkinT29J-zbp4-3l1fl9c3nL5cfrktbt5BK5Er0SpJq0fJNZ21bc4kWqJaNkJ2tlLKKuFIGQHHqN9jJDVSNElmfAaxO2PvD3Lt5M1KXd6dgBn0X3GjCg_bG6T9_JrfTW3-vhRSIos0D3j0NCP77TDHp0WUJw2Am8nPUqBanqlr-ByolCMy6moy-_Qvd-zlM2YlMCYXQ1ACZEgfKBh9joP75bgS9JKz3-uC6XhJeqjnh3Pbmd83PTT8j_WUKZefvHQUdrVtS6Vwgm3Tn3b83PAJ0YrlV</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1648107500</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>von Stumm, Sophie ; Plomin, Robert</creator><creatorcontrib>von Stumm, Sophie ; Plomin, Robert</creatorcontrib><description>Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations between SES and individual differences in the intelligence starting point (intercept) and in the rate and direction of change in scores (slope and quadratic term) from infancy through adolescence in 14,853 children from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), assessed 9 times on IQ between the ages of 2 and 16years. SES was significantly associated with intelligence growth factors: higher SES was related both to a higher starting point in infancy and to greater gains in intelligence over time. Specifically, children from low SES families scored on average 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES backgrounds; by age 16, this difference had almost tripled. Although these key results did not vary across girls and boys, we observed gender differences in the development of intelligence in early childhood. Overall, SES was shown to be associated with individual differences in intercepts as well as slopes of intelligence. However, this finding does not warrant causal interpretations of the relationship between SES and the development of intelligence.
•IQ growth trajectories were modeled in British children from age 2 to 16years.•Children's socioeconomic background (SES) was associated with IQ growth.•High and low SES children differed by 6 IQ points at age 2.•By age 16, this IQ difference between high and low SES children had tripled.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0160-2896</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7935</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002</identifier><identifier>PMID: 26640306</identifier><identifier>CODEN: NTLLDT</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Age differences ; Child development ; Gender ; Gender differences ; Growth models ; Intelligence ; Intelligence tests ; Latent growth ; Socioeconomic factors ; Socioeconomic status</subject><ispartof>Intelligence (Norwood), 2015-01, Vol.48, p.30-36</ispartof><rights>2014 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright Ablex Publishing Corporation Jan-Feb 2015</rights><rights>2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. 2014 Elsevier Inc.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925,30999</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26640306$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>von Stumm, Sophie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Plomin, Robert</creatorcontrib><title>Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence</title><title>Intelligence (Norwood)</title><addtitle>Intelligence</addtitle><description>Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations between SES and individual differences in the intelligence starting point (intercept) and in the rate and direction of change in scores (slope and quadratic term) from infancy through adolescence in 14,853 children from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), assessed 9 times on IQ between the ages of 2 and 16years. SES was significantly associated with intelligence growth factors: higher SES was related both to a higher starting point in infancy and to greater gains in intelligence over time. Specifically, children from low SES families scored on average 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES backgrounds; by age 16, this difference had almost tripled. Although these key results did not vary across girls and boys, we observed gender differences in the development of intelligence in early childhood. Overall, SES was shown to be associated with individual differences in intercepts as well as slopes of intelligence. However, this finding does not warrant causal interpretations of the relationship between SES and the development of intelligence.
•IQ growth trajectories were modeled in British children from age 2 to 16years.•Children's socioeconomic background (SES) was associated with IQ growth.•High and low SES children differed by 6 IQ points at age 2.•By age 16, this IQ difference between high and low SES children had tripled.</description><subject>Age differences</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Gender differences</subject><subject>Growth models</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>Intelligence tests</subject><subject>Latent growth</subject><subject>Socioeconomic factors</subject><subject>Socioeconomic status</subject><issn>0160-2896</issn><issn>1873-7935</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2015</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkUtr3DAUhUVoaaZJ_kEohm668eReWZblTaGEtikEsmi6Fhr5ekaDbaWSnJJ_H5lJ08eidCW4-u7jnMPYOcIaAeXFfu2mRMOw5oAil9YA_IitUDVV2bRV_YKtMgYlV608Zq9j3ANAjUK9YsdcSgEVyBW7_eqt82T95Edni5hMmmNhpq5IOyq2wf9Iu8L3xWGX29JkqeiDH3OlN5N9yFzw83ZXmM4PFO0CnLKXvRkinT29J-zbp4-3l1fl9c3nL5cfrktbt5BK5Er0SpJq0fJNZ21bc4kWqJaNkJ2tlLKKuFIGQHHqN9jJDVSNElmfAaxO2PvD3Lt5M1KXd6dgBn0X3GjCg_bG6T9_JrfTW3-vhRSIos0D3j0NCP77TDHp0WUJw2Am8nPUqBanqlr-ByolCMy6moy-_Qvd-zlM2YlMCYXQ1ACZEgfKBh9joP75bgS9JKz3-uC6XhJeqjnh3Pbmd83PTT8j_WUKZefvHQUdrVtS6Vwgm3Tn3b83PAJ0YrlV</recordid><startdate>201501</startdate><enddate>201501</enddate><creator>von Stumm, Sophie</creator><creator>Plomin, Robert</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>6I.</scope><scope>AAFTH</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201501</creationdate><title>Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence</title><author>von Stumm, Sophie ; Plomin, Robert</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2015</creationdate><topic>Age differences</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Gender differences</topic><topic>Growth models</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>Intelligence tests</topic><topic>Latent growth</topic><topic>Socioeconomic factors</topic><topic>Socioeconomic status</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>von Stumm, Sophie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Plomin, Robert</creatorcontrib><collection>ScienceDirect Open Access Titles</collection><collection>Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>von Stumm, Sophie</au><au>Plomin, Robert</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence</atitle><jtitle>Intelligence (Norwood)</jtitle><addtitle>Intelligence</addtitle><date>2015-01</date><risdate>2015</risdate><volume>48</volume><spage>30</spage><epage>36</epage><pages>30-36</pages><issn>0160-2896</issn><eissn>1873-7935</eissn><coden>NTLLDT</coden><abstract>Low socioeconomic status (SES) children perform on average worse on intelligence tests than children from higher SES backgrounds, but the developmental relationship between intelligence and SES has not been adequately investigated. Here, we use latent growth curve (LGC) models to assess associations between SES and individual differences in the intelligence starting point (intercept) and in the rate and direction of change in scores (slope and quadratic term) from infancy through adolescence in 14,853 children from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), assessed 9 times on IQ between the ages of 2 and 16years. SES was significantly associated with intelligence growth factors: higher SES was related both to a higher starting point in infancy and to greater gains in intelligence over time. Specifically, children from low SES families scored on average 6 IQ points lower at age 2 than children from high SES backgrounds; by age 16, this difference had almost tripled. Although these key results did not vary across girls and boys, we observed gender differences in the development of intelligence in early childhood. Overall, SES was shown to be associated with individual differences in intercepts as well as slopes of intelligence. However, this finding does not warrant causal interpretations of the relationship between SES and the development of intelligence.
•IQ growth trajectories were modeled in British children from age 2 to 16years.•Children's socioeconomic background (SES) was associated with IQ growth.•High and low SES children differed by 6 IQ points at age 2.•By age 16, this IQ difference between high and low SES children had tripled.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>26640306</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002</doi><tpages>7</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0160-2896 |
ispartof | Intelligence (Norwood), 2015-01, Vol.48, p.30-36 |
issn | 0160-2896 1873-7935 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4641149 |
source | Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); ScienceDirect Journals |
subjects | Age differences Child development Gender Gender differences Growth models Intelligence Intelligence tests Latent growth Socioeconomic factors Socioeconomic status |
title | Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-28T10%3A37%3A59IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Socioeconomic%20status%20and%20the%20growth%20of%20intelligence%20from%20infancy%20through%20adolescence&rft.jtitle=Intelligence%20(Norwood)&rft.au=von%20Stumm,%20Sophie&rft.date=2015-01&rft.volume=48&rft.spage=30&rft.epage=36&rft.pages=30-36&rft.issn=0160-2896&rft.eissn=1873-7935&rft.coden=NTLLDT&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1660415907%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c590t-1284f86e891c2bdcc95261c0e56746dc388c8e288a0082efb1d6b03784793a013%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1648107500&rft_id=info:pmid/26640306&rfr_iscdi=true |