Loading…
Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia
Over the last years, several studies have suggested a possible link between dyslexia and deficits in low-level visual processing (e.g., excessive crowding). At the same time, specially designed “dyslexia-friendly” fonts appeared on the market. This class of fonts presents two main features: the part...
Saved in:
Published in: | Annals of dyslexia 2020-04, Vol.70 (1), p.141-152 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573 |
container_end_page | 152 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 141 |
container_title | Annals of dyslexia |
container_volume | 70 |
creator | Galliussi, Jessica Perondi, Luciano Chia, Giuseppe Gerbino, Walter Bernardis, Paolo |
description | Over the last years, several studies have suggested a possible link between dyslexia and deficits in low-level visual processing (e.g., excessive crowding). At the same time, specially designed “dyslexia-friendly” fonts appeared on the market. This class of fonts presents two main features: the particular graphic characteristics of the letterform designed to avoid confusion between similarly shaped letters, and wider inter-letter and inter-word spacing to limit crowding. The literature testing the efficacy of “dyslexia-friendly” fonts in improving reading accuracy and increasing reading speed is controversial. We evaluated the impact of letterform (with vs. without dyslexia-friendly graphic features), inter-letter spacing (standard vs. increased), and inter-word spacing (standard vs. increased) on reading accuracy and speed. Two groups of 64 children each, with and without dyslexia, read aloud 8 equivalent texts. The data collected failed to show any effect from the letterform. As regards spacing, the data showed that reading speed is impaired by an increase in inter-letter spacing not combined with an adequate increase in inter-word spacing. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s11881-020-00194-x |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7188700</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1251705</ericid><sourcerecordid>2395450167</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9Uc1u1DAQthCIbgsvgASKxKUHDHYc2zEHJFQVKKrEBc6W40y2rrJ2sB26-xY8Mt6mbIEDp7Hm-5kZfwg9o-Q1JUS-SZS2LcWkJpgQqhq8fYBWVLEGy7phD9GKSCawYq08QscpXZMiUrJ5jI5YTQtFyBX6eeEzRDxCLqVKk7HOr19V7rZ7E2J_3zO-r4bgc3Xj8lXV79IIW2fwEB34ftxVA5g8R0hvqwwpF0mp21xFML3p3OjyrthWE4RphMVj77h_hDkf_J6gR4MZEzy9qyfo24fzr2ef8OWXjxdn7y-xbZTIuBZCsFZRQWsruSB86AbeKMsk6S2IWvBOqY4DtbLpO257CU0Dg-VABJNcshP0bvGd5m4DReNzNKOeotuYuNPBOP034t2VXocfWpZPl4QUg9M7gxi-z-VivXHJwjgaD2FOumZSipYw0Rbqy3-o12GOvpxXWIo3nFCx36heWDaGlCIMh2Uo0fvA9RK4LoHr28D1tohe_HnGQfI74UJ4vhAgOnuAzz_TmlNJeMHZgqeC-TXE-93-M_YX5frDtw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2395450167</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia</title><source>JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection</source><source>Springer Nature</source><source>Linguistics Collection</source><source>ERIC</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><source>Education Collection</source><creator>Galliussi, Jessica ; Perondi, Luciano ; Chia, Giuseppe ; Gerbino, Walter ; Bernardis, Paolo</creator><creatorcontrib>Galliussi, Jessica ; Perondi, Luciano ; Chia, Giuseppe ; Gerbino, Walter ; Bernardis, Paolo</creatorcontrib><description>Over the last years, several studies have suggested a possible link between dyslexia and deficits in low-level visual processing (e.g., excessive crowding). At the same time, specially designed “dyslexia-friendly” fonts appeared on the market. This class of fonts presents two main features: the particular graphic characteristics of the letterform designed to avoid confusion between similarly shaped letters, and wider inter-letter and inter-word spacing to limit crowding. The literature testing the efficacy of “dyslexia-friendly” fonts in improving reading accuracy and increasing reading speed is controversial. We evaluated the impact of letterform (with vs. without dyslexia-friendly graphic features), inter-letter spacing (standard vs. increased), and inter-word spacing (standard vs. increased) on reading accuracy and speed. Two groups of 64 children each, with and without dyslexia, read aloud 8 equivalent texts. The data collected failed to show any effect from the letterform. As regards spacing, the data showed that reading speed is impaired by an increase in inter-letter spacing not combined with an adequate increase in inter-word spacing.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0736-9387</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1934-7243</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s11881-020-00194-x</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32172467</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Springer US</publisher><subject>Alphabets ; Child ; Children ; Comprehension - physiology ; Crowding ; Design ; Dyslexia ; Dyslexia - psychology ; Dyslexia - therapy ; Education ; Female ; Humans ; Language and Literature ; Layout (Publications) ; Linguistics ; Male ; Neurology ; Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology ; Photic Stimulation - methods ; Proximity ; Psycholinguistics ; Readability ; Reading ; Reading accuracy ; Reading Improvement ; Reading Rate ; Social Sciences ; Typefaces ; Visual Literacy ; Visual Perception - physiology ; Visual processing ; Word Processing</subject><ispartof>Annals of dyslexia, 2020-04, Vol.70 (1), p.141-152</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2020</rights><rights>The Author(s) 2020. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-4010-4694 ; 0000-0002-7198-4030 ; 0000-0002-1883-771X ; 0000-0001-9476-8480</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2395450167/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/2395450167?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,12850,21377,21381,21393,27923,27924,31268,33610,33611,33876,33877,33910,33911,43732,43879,43895,73992,74168,74184</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1251705$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32172467$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Galliussi, Jessica</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Perondi, Luciano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chia, Giuseppe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gerbino, Walter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernardis, Paolo</creatorcontrib><title>Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia</title><title>Annals of dyslexia</title><addtitle>Ann. of Dyslexia</addtitle><addtitle>Ann Dyslexia</addtitle><description>Over the last years, several studies have suggested a possible link between dyslexia and deficits in low-level visual processing (e.g., excessive crowding). At the same time, specially designed “dyslexia-friendly” fonts appeared on the market. This class of fonts presents two main features: the particular graphic characteristics of the letterform designed to avoid confusion between similarly shaped letters, and wider inter-letter and inter-word spacing to limit crowding. The literature testing the efficacy of “dyslexia-friendly” fonts in improving reading accuracy and increasing reading speed is controversial. We evaluated the impact of letterform (with vs. without dyslexia-friendly graphic features), inter-letter spacing (standard vs. increased), and inter-word spacing (standard vs. increased) on reading accuracy and speed. Two groups of 64 children each, with and without dyslexia, read aloud 8 equivalent texts. The data collected failed to show any effect from the letterform. As regards spacing, the data showed that reading speed is impaired by an increase in inter-letter spacing not combined with an adequate increase in inter-word spacing.</description><subject>Alphabets</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Comprehension - physiology</subject><subject>Crowding</subject><subject>Design</subject><subject>Dyslexia</subject><subject>Dyslexia - psychology</subject><subject>Dyslexia - therapy</subject><subject>Education</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language and Literature</subject><subject>Layout (Publications)</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation - methods</subject><subject>Proximity</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Readability</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Reading accuracy</subject><subject>Reading Improvement</subject><subject>Reading Rate</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Typefaces</subject><subject>Visual Literacy</subject><subject>Visual Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Visual processing</subject><subject>Word Processing</subject><issn>0736-9387</issn><issn>1934-7243</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CJNVE</sourceid><sourceid>CPGLG</sourceid><sourceid>M0P</sourceid><recordid>eNp9Uc1u1DAQthCIbgsvgASKxKUHDHYc2zEHJFQVKKrEBc6W40y2rrJ2sB26-xY8Mt6mbIEDp7Hm-5kZfwg9o-Q1JUS-SZS2LcWkJpgQqhq8fYBWVLEGy7phD9GKSCawYq08QscpXZMiUrJ5jI5YTQtFyBX6eeEzRDxCLqVKk7HOr19V7rZ7E2J_3zO-r4bgc3Xj8lXV79IIW2fwEB34ftxVA5g8R0hvqwwpF0mp21xFML3p3OjyrthWE4RphMVj77h_hDkf_J6gR4MZEzy9qyfo24fzr2ef8OWXjxdn7y-xbZTIuBZCsFZRQWsruSB86AbeKMsk6S2IWvBOqY4DtbLpO257CU0Dg-VABJNcshP0bvGd5m4DReNzNKOeotuYuNPBOP034t2VXocfWpZPl4QUg9M7gxi-z-VivXHJwjgaD2FOumZSipYw0Rbqy3-o12GOvpxXWIo3nFCx36heWDaGlCIMh2Uo0fvA9RK4LoHr28D1tohe_HnGQfI74UJ4vhAgOnuAzz_TmlNJeMHZgqeC-TXE-93-M_YX5frDtw</recordid><startdate>20200401</startdate><enddate>20200401</enddate><creator>Galliussi, Jessica</creator><creator>Perondi, Luciano</creator><creator>Chia, Giuseppe</creator><creator>Gerbino, Walter</creator><creator>Bernardis, Paolo</creator><general>Springer US</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88B</scope><scope>88C</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88I</scope><scope>8AF</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CJNVE</scope><scope>CPGLG</scope><scope>CRLPW</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0P</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M0T</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2P</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEDU</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>S0X</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4010-4694</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7198-4030</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1883-771X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9476-8480</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200401</creationdate><title>Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia</title><author>Galliussi, Jessica ; Perondi, Luciano ; Chia, Giuseppe ; Gerbino, Walter ; Bernardis, Paolo</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Alphabets</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Comprehension - physiology</topic><topic>Crowding</topic><topic>Design</topic><topic>Dyslexia</topic><topic>Dyslexia - psychology</topic><topic>Dyslexia - therapy</topic><topic>Education</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language and Literature</topic><topic>Layout (Publications)</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation - methods</topic><topic>Proximity</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Readability</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Reading accuracy</topic><topic>Reading Improvement</topic><topic>Reading Rate</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Typefaces</topic><topic>Visual Literacy</topic><topic>Visual Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Visual processing</topic><topic>Word Processing</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Galliussi, Jessica</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Perondi, Luciano</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Chia, Giuseppe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gerbino, Walter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bernardis, Paolo</creatorcontrib><collection>SpringerOpen (Open Access)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Education Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Healthcare Administration Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>STEM Database</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Education Collection</collection><collection>Linguistics Collection</collection><collection>Linguistics Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Education Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Healthcare Administration Database</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Research Library</collection><collection>ProQuest Science Journals</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Education</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>SIRS Editorial</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Annals of dyslexia</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Galliussi, Jessica</au><au>Perondi, Luciano</au><au>Chia, Giuseppe</au><au>Gerbino, Walter</au><au>Bernardis, Paolo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1251705</ericid><atitle>Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia</atitle><jtitle>Annals of dyslexia</jtitle><stitle>Ann. of Dyslexia</stitle><addtitle>Ann Dyslexia</addtitle><date>2020-04-01</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>70</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>141</spage><epage>152</epage><pages>141-152</pages><issn>0736-9387</issn><eissn>1934-7243</eissn><abstract>Over the last years, several studies have suggested a possible link between dyslexia and deficits in low-level visual processing (e.g., excessive crowding). At the same time, specially designed “dyslexia-friendly” fonts appeared on the market. This class of fonts presents two main features: the particular graphic characteristics of the letterform designed to avoid confusion between similarly shaped letters, and wider inter-letter and inter-word spacing to limit crowding. The literature testing the efficacy of “dyslexia-friendly” fonts in improving reading accuracy and increasing reading speed is controversial. We evaluated the impact of letterform (with vs. without dyslexia-friendly graphic features), inter-letter spacing (standard vs. increased), and inter-word spacing (standard vs. increased) on reading accuracy and speed. Two groups of 64 children each, with and without dyslexia, read aloud 8 equivalent texts. The data collected failed to show any effect from the letterform. As regards spacing, the data showed that reading speed is impaired by an increase in inter-letter spacing not combined with an adequate increase in inter-word spacing.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Springer US</pub><pmid>32172467</pmid><doi>10.1007/s11881-020-00194-x</doi><tpages>12</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4010-4694</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7198-4030</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1883-771X</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9476-8480</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0736-9387 |
ispartof | Annals of dyslexia, 2020-04, Vol.70 (1), p.141-152 |
issn | 0736-9387 1934-7243 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7188700 |
source | JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection; Social Science Premium Collection; Springer Nature; Linguistics Collection; ERIC; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA); Education Collection |
subjects | Alphabets Child Children Comprehension - physiology Crowding Design Dyslexia Dyslexia - psychology Dyslexia - therapy Education Female Humans Language and Literature Layout (Publications) Linguistics Male Neurology Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology Photic Stimulation - methods Proximity Psycholinguistics Readability Reading Reading accuracy Reading Improvement Reading Rate Social Sciences Typefaces Visual Literacy Visual Perception - physiology Visual processing Word Processing |
title | Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-11T03%3A23%3A39IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Inter-letter%20spacing,%20inter-word%20spacing,%20and%20font%20with%20dyslexia-friendly%20features:%20testing%20text%20readability%20in%20people%20with%20and%20without%20dyslexia&rft.jtitle=Annals%20of%20dyslexia&rft.au=Galliussi,%20Jessica&rft.date=2020-04-01&rft.volume=70&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=141&rft.epage=152&rft.pages=141-152&rft.issn=0736-9387&rft.eissn=1934-7243&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s11881-020-00194-x&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2395450167%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c496t-26663891612c75605fbf549c370dce6265b99b5e1c74db5cd7e44efc5e0637573%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2395450167&rft_id=info:pmid/32172467&rft_ericid=EJ1251705&rfr_iscdi=true |