Loading…

Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control

According to Fitts (1954), movement time (MT) is a function of the combined effects of movement amplitude and target width (index of difficulty). Aiming movements with the same index of difficulty and MT may have different planning and control processes depending on the specific combination of movem...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology Human experimental psychology, 1987-11, Vol.39 (4), p.629-647
Main Authors: MacKenzie, C. L., Marteniuk, R. G., Dugas, C., Liske, D., Eickmeier, B.
Format: Article
Language:English
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93
container_end_page 647
container_issue 4
container_start_page 629
container_title The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology
container_volume 39
creator MacKenzie, C. L.
Marteniuk, R. G.
Dugas, C.
Liske, D.
Eickmeier, B.
description According to Fitts (1954), movement time (MT) is a function of the combined effects of movement amplitude and target width (index of difficulty). Aiming movements with the same index of difficulty and MT may have different planning and control processes depending on the specific combination of movement amplitude and target size. Trajectories were evaluated for a broad range of amplitudes and target sizes. A three-dimensional motion recording system (WATSMART) monitored the position of a stylus during aiming movements. MT results replicated Fitts' Law. Analysis of the resultant velocity profiles indicated the following significant effects: As amplitude of movement increased, so did the time to peak resultant velocity; peak resultant velocity increased slightly with target size, and to a greater extent with increases in the amplitude of movement; the time after peak resultant velocity was a function of both amplitude and target size. Resultant velocity profiles were normalized in the time domain to look for scalar relation in the trajectory shape. This revealed that: the resultant velocity profiles were not symmetrical; the proportion of time spent prior to and after peak speed was sensitive to target size only, i.e. as target size decreased, the profiles became more skewed to the right, indicating a longer decelerative phase; for a given target size, a family of curves might be defined and scaled on movement amplitude. These results suggest that a generalized program (base trajectory representation) exists for a given target width and is parameterized or scaled according to the amplitude of movement.
doi_str_mv 10.1080/14640748708401806
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_sage_</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_journals_1297867386</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1080_14640748708401806</sage_id><sourcerecordid>1297867386</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kE9LAzEQxYMoWKsfwFvAg6etyW7IH_EixWqhoId6DtnsrKbubmqSKv32ptSDIDqXYR7v9xgeQueUTCiR5IoyzohgUhDJCJWEH6DRTiuySA7RiJSiLJiS4hidxLgieRgjI_S0fA0AReN6GKLzg-lw7z8gXwmnYFZgkw8OInYDnrmU4iVOJr5d43m_7pw1KTMRtz5g64cUfHeKjlrTRTj73mP0PLtbTh-KxeP9fHq7KGzFeCoqJbi1VpU1Nw2ldSlY1TQNqUEpoFZJCVCCldKQtpWmqqUUkgBXVDDDQVVjdLHPXQf_voGY9MpvQv4_aloqIbmoJM8uunfZ4GMM0Op1cL0JW02J3hWnfxWXmcmeieYFfqT-A9zsATfkInrz6UPX6GS2nQ9tMIN1UVd_418QQIDT</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1297867386</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control</title><source>Taylor &amp; Francis Behavioral Science Archive 2015</source><creator>MacKenzie, C. L. ; Marteniuk, R. G. ; Dugas, C. ; Liske, D. ; Eickmeier, B.</creator><creatorcontrib>MacKenzie, C. L. ; Marteniuk, R. G. ; Dugas, C. ; Liske, D. ; Eickmeier, B.</creatorcontrib><description>According to Fitts (1954), movement time (MT) is a function of the combined effects of movement amplitude and target width (index of difficulty). Aiming movements with the same index of difficulty and MT may have different planning and control processes depending on the specific combination of movement amplitude and target size. Trajectories were evaluated for a broad range of amplitudes and target sizes. A three-dimensional motion recording system (WATSMART) monitored the position of a stylus during aiming movements. MT results replicated Fitts' Law. Analysis of the resultant velocity profiles indicated the following significant effects: As amplitude of movement increased, so did the time to peak resultant velocity; peak resultant velocity increased slightly with target size, and to a greater extent with increases in the amplitude of movement; the time after peak resultant velocity was a function of both amplitude and target size. Resultant velocity profiles were normalized in the time domain to look for scalar relation in the trajectory shape. This revealed that: the resultant velocity profiles were not symmetrical; the proportion of time spent prior to and after peak speed was sensitive to target size only, i.e. as target size decreased, the profiles became more skewed to the right, indicating a longer decelerative phase; for a given target size, a family of curves might be defined and scaled on movement amplitude. These results suggest that a generalized program (base trajectory representation) exists for a given target width and is parameterized or scaled according to the amplitude of movement.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0272-4987</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1464-0740</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1080/14640748708401806</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>London, England: Taylor &amp; Francis Group</publisher><ispartof>The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology, 1987-11, Vol.39 (4), p.629-647</ispartof><rights>Copyright The Experimental Psychology Society 1987</rights><rights>1987 Experimental Pscyhology Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/14640748708401806$$EPDF$$P50$$Ginformaworld$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/14640748708401806$$EHTML$$P50$$Ginformaworld$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,59779,60568</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>MacKenzie, C. L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marteniuk, R. G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dugas, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liske, D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Eickmeier, B.</creatorcontrib><title>Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control</title><title>The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology</title><description>According to Fitts (1954), movement time (MT) is a function of the combined effects of movement amplitude and target width (index of difficulty). Aiming movements with the same index of difficulty and MT may have different planning and control processes depending on the specific combination of movement amplitude and target size. Trajectories were evaluated for a broad range of amplitudes and target sizes. A three-dimensional motion recording system (WATSMART) monitored the position of a stylus during aiming movements. MT results replicated Fitts' Law. Analysis of the resultant velocity profiles indicated the following significant effects: As amplitude of movement increased, so did the time to peak resultant velocity; peak resultant velocity increased slightly with target size, and to a greater extent with increases in the amplitude of movement; the time after peak resultant velocity was a function of both amplitude and target size. Resultant velocity profiles were normalized in the time domain to look for scalar relation in the trajectory shape. This revealed that: the resultant velocity profiles were not symmetrical; the proportion of time spent prior to and after peak speed was sensitive to target size only, i.e. as target size decreased, the profiles became more skewed to the right, indicating a longer decelerative phase; for a given target size, a family of curves might be defined and scaled on movement amplitude. These results suggest that a generalized program (base trajectory representation) exists for a given target width and is parameterized or scaled according to the amplitude of movement.</description><issn>0272-4987</issn><issn>1464-0740</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1987</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kE9LAzEQxYMoWKsfwFvAg6etyW7IH_EixWqhoId6DtnsrKbubmqSKv32ptSDIDqXYR7v9xgeQueUTCiR5IoyzohgUhDJCJWEH6DRTiuySA7RiJSiLJiS4hidxLgieRgjI_S0fA0AReN6GKLzg-lw7z8gXwmnYFZgkw8OInYDnrmU4iVOJr5d43m_7pw1KTMRtz5g64cUfHeKjlrTRTj73mP0PLtbTh-KxeP9fHq7KGzFeCoqJbi1VpU1Nw2ldSlY1TQNqUEpoFZJCVCCldKQtpWmqqUUkgBXVDDDQVVjdLHPXQf_voGY9MpvQv4_aloqIbmoJM8uunfZ4GMM0Op1cL0JW02J3hWnfxWXmcmeieYFfqT-A9zsATfkInrz6UPX6GS2nQ9tMIN1UVd_418QQIDT</recordid><startdate>19871101</startdate><enddate>19871101</enddate><creator>MacKenzie, C. L.</creator><creator>Marteniuk, R. G.</creator><creator>Dugas, C.</creator><creator>Liske, D.</creator><creator>Eickmeier, B.</creator><general>Taylor &amp; Francis Group</general><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>Academic Press for the Experimental Psychology Society</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>GPCCI</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19871101</creationdate><title>Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control</title><author>MacKenzie, C. L. ; Marteniuk, R. G. ; Dugas, C. ; Liske, D. ; Eickmeier, B.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1987</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>MacKenzie, C. L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marteniuk, R. G.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dugas, C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liske, D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Eickmeier, B.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 10</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><jtitle>The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>MacKenzie, C. L.</au><au>Marteniuk, R. G.</au><au>Dugas, C.</au><au>Liske, D.</au><au>Eickmeier, B.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control</atitle><jtitle>The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology</jtitle><date>1987-11-01</date><risdate>1987</risdate><volume>39</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>629</spage><epage>647</epage><pages>629-647</pages><issn>0272-4987</issn><eissn>1464-0740</eissn><abstract>According to Fitts (1954), movement time (MT) is a function of the combined effects of movement amplitude and target width (index of difficulty). Aiming movements with the same index of difficulty and MT may have different planning and control processes depending on the specific combination of movement amplitude and target size. Trajectories were evaluated for a broad range of amplitudes and target sizes. A three-dimensional motion recording system (WATSMART) monitored the position of a stylus during aiming movements. MT results replicated Fitts' Law. Analysis of the resultant velocity profiles indicated the following significant effects: As amplitude of movement increased, so did the time to peak resultant velocity; peak resultant velocity increased slightly with target size, and to a greater extent with increases in the amplitude of movement; the time after peak resultant velocity was a function of both amplitude and target size. Resultant velocity profiles were normalized in the time domain to look for scalar relation in the trajectory shape. This revealed that: the resultant velocity profiles were not symmetrical; the proportion of time spent prior to and after peak speed was sensitive to target size only, i.e. as target size decreased, the profiles became more skewed to the right, indicating a longer decelerative phase; for a given target size, a family of curves might be defined and scaled on movement amplitude. These results suggest that a generalized program (base trajectory representation) exists for a given target width and is parameterized or scaled according to the amplitude of movement.</abstract><cop>London, England</cop><pub>Taylor &amp; Francis Group</pub><doi>10.1080/14640748708401806</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0272-4987
ispartof The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology, 1987-11, Vol.39 (4), p.629-647
issn 0272-4987
1464-0740
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_journals_1297867386
source Taylor & Francis Behavioral Science Archive 2015
title Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts' task: Implications for control
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T03%3A15%3A26IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_sage_&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Three-dimensional%20movement%20trajectories%20in%20Fitts'%20task:%20Implications%20for%20control&rft.jtitle=The%20Quarterly%20journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology.%20A,%20Human%20experimental%20psychology&rft.au=MacKenzie,%20C.%20L.&rft.date=1987-11-01&rft.volume=39&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=629&rft.epage=647&rft.pages=629-647&rft.issn=0272-4987&rft.eissn=1464-0740&rft_id=info:doi/10.1080/14640748708401806&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_sage_%3E1297867386%3C/proquest_sage_%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c346t-3976ccc92b6ad11b2743ddd0be99e1c988ee2ec88a0ff8a3b88780e69174a6e93%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1297867386&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_sage_id=10.1080_14640748708401806&rfr_iscdi=true