Loading…

Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development

A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Developmental science 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.e13066-n/a
Main Authors: Motamedi, Yasamin, Murgiano, Margherita, Perniss, Pamela, Wonnacott, Elizabeth, Marshall, Chloë, Goldin‐Meadow, Susan, Vigliocco, Gabriella
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
cited_by cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73
cites cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73
container_end_page n/a
container_issue 3
container_start_page e13066
container_title Developmental science
container_volume 24
creator Motamedi, Yasamin
Murgiano, Margherita
Perniss, Pamela
Wonnacott, Elizabeth
Marshall, Chloë
Goldin‐Meadow, Susan
Vigliocco, Gabriella
description A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning. What role does onomatopoeia play in children's vocabulary development? We use naturalistic observations of caregiver‐child interactions as well as a word learning experiment with 2–3 year old English‐speaking children to understand how caregivers use onomatopoea in interactions with their children, and how children learn onomatopoeia in both situated and displaced learning contexts.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/desc.13066
format article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2464147239</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ1294225</ericid><sourcerecordid>2520801838</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kMtLxDAQxoMovi_elYIXEap5tGnrTdb1xYLgA7yFNJku1TapyVbd_95oVwUP5jIZvt_MfHwI7RB8RMI71uDVEWGY8yW0ThKexRnPs-XwZ5zFWZo-rqEN758wxgnDZBWtMUYZYaxYR7eT2jzXZho10kx7OYVoZiMPxls3j-C9A1eDUXAS3RjbypntLNQyqk0E0jXz3ykNr9DYrgUz20IrlWw8bC_qJno4H9-PLuPJzcXV6HQSq4SkPOYSKoU55DSY4rKsdKoZVaqivOQ5zXSZkhwkUxowZGWRhlYXWCstgedlxjbRwbC3c_alBz8Tbe0VNMET2N4LmvCEJBllRUD3_6BPtncmuBM0pTjHJGd5oA4HSjnrvYNKdK5upZsLgsVn0uIzafGVdID3Fiv7sgX9g35HG4DdAQgRqh95fE1okVCaBp0M-lvdwPyfU-JsfDcajn4A2fGSxQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2520801838</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development</title><source>Wiley</source><source>ERIC</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Motamedi, Yasamin ; Murgiano, Margherita ; Perniss, Pamela ; Wonnacott, Elizabeth ; Marshall, Chloë ; Goldin‐Meadow, Susan ; Vigliocco, Gabriella</creator><creatorcontrib>Motamedi, Yasamin ; Murgiano, Margherita ; Perniss, Pamela ; Wonnacott, Elizabeth ; Marshall, Chloë ; Goldin‐Meadow, Susan ; Vigliocco, Gabriella</creatorcontrib><description>A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning. What role does onomatopoeia play in children's vocabulary development? We use naturalistic observations of caregiver‐child interactions as well as a word learning experiment with 2–3 year old English‐speaking children to understand how caregivers use onomatopoea in interactions with their children, and how children learn onomatopoeia in both situated and displaced learning contexts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1363-755X</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1467-7687</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-7687</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/desc.13066</identifier><identifier>PMID: 33231339</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Wiley</publisher><subject>Caregiver Child Relationship ; Caregivers ; Children ; Children &amp; youth ; child‐directed language ; Early experience ; Figurative Language ; Imagery ; Language ; Language Acquisition ; Onomatopoeia ; Phonology ; Sensory Experience ; sound symbolism ; Vocabulary Development ; Vocabulary learning ; word learning ; Word meaning</subject><ispartof>Developmental science, 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.e13066-n/a</ispartof><rights>2020 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd</rights><rights>2020 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2021 John Wiley &amp; Sons Ltd</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73</cites><orcidid>0000-0003-2405-1999</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,31269</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1294225$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33231339$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Motamedi, Yasamin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Murgiano, Margherita</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Perniss, Pamela</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wonnacott, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marshall, Chloë</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Goldin‐Meadow, Susan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vigliocco, Gabriella</creatorcontrib><title>Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development</title><title>Developmental science</title><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><description>A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning. What role does onomatopoeia play in children's vocabulary development? We use naturalistic observations of caregiver‐child interactions as well as a word learning experiment with 2–3 year old English‐speaking children to understand how caregivers use onomatopoea in interactions with their children, and how children learn onomatopoeia in both situated and displaced learning contexts.</description><subject>Caregiver Child Relationship</subject><subject>Caregivers</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children &amp; youth</subject><subject>child‐directed language</subject><subject>Early experience</subject><subject>Figurative Language</subject><subject>Imagery</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Acquisition</subject><subject>Onomatopoeia</subject><subject>Phonology</subject><subject>Sensory Experience</subject><subject>sound symbolism</subject><subject>Vocabulary Development</subject><subject>Vocabulary learning</subject><subject>word learning</subject><subject>Word meaning</subject><issn>1363-755X</issn><issn>1467-7687</issn><issn>1467-7687</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2021</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7SW</sourceid><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kMtLxDAQxoMovi_elYIXEap5tGnrTdb1xYLgA7yFNJku1TapyVbd_95oVwUP5jIZvt_MfHwI7RB8RMI71uDVEWGY8yW0ThKexRnPs-XwZ5zFWZo-rqEN758wxgnDZBWtMUYZYaxYR7eT2jzXZho10kx7OYVoZiMPxls3j-C9A1eDUXAS3RjbypntLNQyqk0E0jXz3ykNr9DYrgUz20IrlWw8bC_qJno4H9-PLuPJzcXV6HQSq4SkPOYSKoU55DSY4rKsdKoZVaqivOQ5zXSZkhwkUxowZGWRhlYXWCstgedlxjbRwbC3c_alBz8Tbe0VNMET2N4LmvCEJBllRUD3_6BPtncmuBM0pTjHJGd5oA4HSjnrvYNKdK5upZsLgsVn0uIzafGVdID3Fiv7sgX9g35HG4DdAQgRqh95fE1okVCaBp0M-lvdwPyfU-JsfDcajn4A2fGSxQ</recordid><startdate>202105</startdate><enddate>202105</enddate><creator>Motamedi, Yasamin</creator><creator>Murgiano, Margherita</creator><creator>Perniss, Pamela</creator><creator>Wonnacott, Elizabeth</creator><creator>Marshall, Chloë</creator><creator>Goldin‐Meadow, Susan</creator><creator>Vigliocco, Gabriella</creator><general>Wiley</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2405-1999</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202105</creationdate><title>Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development</title><author>Motamedi, Yasamin ; Murgiano, Margherita ; Perniss, Pamela ; Wonnacott, Elizabeth ; Marshall, Chloë ; Goldin‐Meadow, Susan ; Vigliocco, Gabriella</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2021</creationdate><topic>Caregiver Child Relationship</topic><topic>Caregivers</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children &amp; youth</topic><topic>child‐directed language</topic><topic>Early experience</topic><topic>Figurative Language</topic><topic>Imagery</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Acquisition</topic><topic>Onomatopoeia</topic><topic>Phonology</topic><topic>Sensory Experience</topic><topic>sound symbolism</topic><topic>Vocabulary Development</topic><topic>Vocabulary learning</topic><topic>word learning</topic><topic>Word meaning</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Motamedi, Yasamin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Murgiano, Margherita</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Perniss, Pamela</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wonnacott, Elizabeth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marshall, Chloë</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Goldin‐Meadow, Susan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vigliocco, Gabriella</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Calcium &amp; Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Motamedi, Yasamin</au><au>Murgiano, Margherita</au><au>Perniss, Pamela</au><au>Wonnacott, Elizabeth</au><au>Marshall, Chloë</au><au>Goldin‐Meadow, Susan</au><au>Vigliocco, Gabriella</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1294225</ericid><atitle>Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development</atitle><jtitle>Developmental science</jtitle><addtitle>Dev Sci</addtitle><date>2021-05</date><risdate>2021</risdate><volume>24</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>e13066</spage><epage>n/a</epage><pages>e13066-n/a</pages><issn>1363-755X</issn><issn>1467-7687</issn><eissn>1467-7687</eissn><abstract>A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning. What role does onomatopoeia play in children's vocabulary development? We use naturalistic observations of caregiver‐child interactions as well as a word learning experiment with 2–3 year old English‐speaking children to understand how caregivers use onomatopoea in interactions with their children, and how children learn onomatopoeia in both situated and displaced learning contexts.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Wiley</pub><pmid>33231339</pmid><doi>10.1111/desc.13066</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2405-1999</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1363-755X
ispartof Developmental science, 2021-05, Vol.24 (3), p.e13066-n/a
issn 1363-755X
1467-7687
1467-7687
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2464147239
source Wiley; ERIC; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
subjects Caregiver Child Relationship
Caregivers
Children
Children & youth
child‐directed language
Early experience
Figurative Language
Imagery
Language
Language Acquisition
Onomatopoeia
Phonology
Sensory Experience
sound symbolism
Vocabulary Development
Vocabulary learning
word learning
Word meaning
title Linking language to sensory experience: Onomatopoeia in early language development
url http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-27T04%3A42%3A40IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Linking%20language%20to%20sensory%20experience:%20Onomatopoeia%20in%20early%20language%20development&rft.jtitle=Developmental%20science&rft.au=Motamedi,%20Yasamin&rft.date=2021-05&rft.volume=24&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=e13066&rft.epage=n/a&rft.pages=e13066-n/a&rft.issn=1363-755X&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/desc.13066&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2520801838%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4156-6aefc06e824306abfd5d32ccf26b6827db518ea3cde0e7b95518d90dcdae68b73%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2520801838&rft_id=info:pmid/33231339&rft_ericid=EJ1294225&rfr_iscdi=true