Loading…
Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar
The idea that natural language grammar and planned action are related systems has been implicit in psychological theory for more than a century. However, formal theories in the two domains have tended to look very different. This article argues that both faculties share the formal character of appli...
Saved in:
Published in: | Linguistics and philosophy 2002-12, Vol.25 (5/6), p.723-753 |
---|---|
Main Author: | |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593 |
container_end_page | 753 |
container_issue | 5/6 |
container_start_page | 723 |
container_title | Linguistics and philosophy |
container_volume | 25 |
creator | Steedman, Mark |
description | The idea that natural language grammar and planned action are related systems has been implicit in psychological theory for more than a century. However, formal theories in the two domains have tended to look very different. This article argues that both faculties share the formal character of applicative systems based on operations corresponding to the same two combinatory operations, namely functional composition and type-raising. Viewing them in this way suggests simpler and more cognitively plausible accounts of both systems, and suggests that the language faculty evolved in the species and develops in children by a rather direct adaptation of a more primitive apparatus for planning purposive action in the world by composing affordances of objects or tools. The knowledge representation that underlies such planning is also reflected in the natural language semantics of tense, mood, and aspect, which the paper begins by arguing provides the key to understanding both systems. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1023/a:1020820000972 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85569647</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>25001872</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>25001872</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE1LAzEQhoMoWKtnT0IR9KJrJ8kmm3grRatQ0IOel9k0gS37UZP20H_vSIuiF-cww2Seecm8jJ1zuOMg5BjvqYARQGELccAGXBUyA5XbQzYArlUG9HLMTlJaEiMKawfs5rXBLt2OJiH0cYGd89RgtxhN-7aqO1z3cTuaRWxbjKfsKGCT_Nm-Dtn748Pb9Cmbv8yep5N5hjmoNWUEp603PndBSW4sigo499JhCL7ShddhIQwKAdZpEbTQJq8wcIMolJVDdr3TXcX-Y-PTumzr5HxDP_X9JpVGKW11XvwLSpNTgCbw8g-47DexoyNKY2QBxghJ0HgHudinFH0oV7Gms7clh_LL4nJS_rKYNq72spgcNiGSf3X6WculVrowxF3suGUiP7_nQgFwQzqfh2mCEw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>883708823</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar</title><source>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</source><source>JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection</source><source>Social Science Premium Collection</source><source>Springer Nature</source><source>Linguistics Collection</source><source>ProQuest One Literature</source><source>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</source><creator>Steedman, Mark</creator><creatorcontrib>Steedman, Mark</creatorcontrib><description>The idea that natural language grammar and planned action are related systems has been implicit in psychological theory for more than a century. However, formal theories in the two domains have tended to look very different. This article argues that both faculties share the formal character of applicative systems based on operations corresponding to the same two combinatory operations, namely functional composition and type-raising. Viewing them in this way suggests simpler and more cognitively plausible accounts of both systems, and suggests that the language faculty evolved in the species and develops in children by a rather direct adaptation of a more primitive apparatus for planning purposive action in the world by composing affordances of objects or tools. The knowledge representation that underlies such planning is also reflected in the natural language semantics of tense, mood, and aspect, which the paper begins by arguing provides the key to understanding both systems.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0165-0157</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1573-0549</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1023/a:1020820000972</identifier><identifier>CODEN: LIPHD6</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Heidelberg: Kluwer Academic Publishers</publisher><subject>Artificial intelligence ; Children ; Grammar ; Knowledge ; Language ; Linguistic theory ; Linguistics ; Mathematical functions ; Music composition ; Musical grammar ; Musical notation ; Natural language ; Ontology ; Philosophy ; Philosophy of language. Logic ; Semantics ; Sensory perception ; Tense ; Verbs ; Word order</subject><ispartof>Linguistics and philosophy, 2002-12, Vol.25 (5/6), p.723-753</ispartof><rights>Copyright 2002 Kluwer Academic Publishers</rights><rights>2003 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Kluwer Academic Publishers 2002</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/883708823/fulltextPDF?pq-origsite=primo$$EPDF$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/883708823?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,778,782,12834,12838,21369,21381,27911,27912,31256,31257,33210,33211,33598,33599,33898,33899,43720,43883,58225,58458,62646,62647,62649,62662,73951,73976,74168</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=14365678$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Steedman, Mark</creatorcontrib><title>Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar</title><title>Linguistics and philosophy</title><description>The idea that natural language grammar and planned action are related systems has been implicit in psychological theory for more than a century. However, formal theories in the two domains have tended to look very different. This article argues that both faculties share the formal character of applicative systems based on operations corresponding to the same two combinatory operations, namely functional composition and type-raising. Viewing them in this way suggests simpler and more cognitively plausible accounts of both systems, and suggests that the language faculty evolved in the species and develops in children by a rather direct adaptation of a more primitive apparatus for planning purposive action in the world by composing affordances of objects or tools. The knowledge representation that underlies such planning is also reflected in the natural language semantics of tense, mood, and aspect, which the paper begins by arguing provides the key to understanding both systems.</description><subject>Artificial intelligence</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Knowledge</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Linguistic theory</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Mathematical functions</subject><subject>Music composition</subject><subject>Musical grammar</subject><subject>Musical notation</subject><subject>Natural language</subject><subject>Ontology</subject><subject>Philosophy</subject><subject>Philosophy of language. Logic</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Sensory perception</subject><subject>Tense</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Word order</subject><issn>0165-0157</issn><issn>1573-0549</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2002</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7T9</sourceid><sourceid>8BJ</sourceid><sourceid>AIMQZ</sourceid><sourceid>ALSLI</sourceid><sourceid>CPGLG</sourceid><sourceid>M2R</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkE1LAzEQhoMoWKtnT0IR9KJrJ8kmm3grRatQ0IOel9k0gS37UZP20H_vSIuiF-cww2Seecm8jJ1zuOMg5BjvqYARQGELccAGXBUyA5XbQzYArlUG9HLMTlJaEiMKawfs5rXBLt2OJiH0cYGd89RgtxhN-7aqO1z3cTuaRWxbjKfsKGCT_Nm-Dtn748Pb9Cmbv8yep5N5hjmoNWUEp603PndBSW4sigo499JhCL7ShddhIQwKAdZpEbTQJq8wcIMolJVDdr3TXcX-Y-PTumzr5HxDP_X9JpVGKW11XvwLSpNTgCbw8g-47DexoyNKY2QBxghJ0HgHudinFH0oV7Gms7clh_LL4nJS_rKYNq72spgcNiGSf3X6WculVrowxF3suGUiP7_nQgFwQzqfh2mCEw</recordid><startdate>20021201</startdate><enddate>20021201</enddate><creator>Steedman, Mark</creator><general>Kluwer Academic Publishers</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>AABKS</scope><scope>ABSDQ</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AIMQZ</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CPGLG</scope><scope>CRLPW</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>LIQON</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PADUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>Q9U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20021201</creationdate><title>Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar</title><author>Steedman, Mark</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2002</creationdate><topic>Artificial intelligence</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Knowledge</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Linguistic theory</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Mathematical functions</topic><topic>Music composition</topic><topic>Musical grammar</topic><topic>Musical notation</topic><topic>Natural language</topic><topic>Ontology</topic><topic>Philosophy</topic><topic>Philosophy of language. Logic</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Sensory perception</topic><topic>Tense</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Word order</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Steedman, Mark</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Philosophy Collection</collection><collection>Philosophy Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Linguistics Collection</collection><collection>Linguistics Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Science Journals</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>Research Library China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><jtitle>Linguistics and philosophy</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Steedman, Mark</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar</atitle><jtitle>Linguistics and philosophy</jtitle><date>2002-12-01</date><risdate>2002</risdate><volume>25</volume><issue>5/6</issue><spage>723</spage><epage>753</epage><pages>723-753</pages><issn>0165-0157</issn><eissn>1573-0549</eissn><coden>LIPHD6</coden><abstract>The idea that natural language grammar and planned action are related systems has been implicit in psychological theory for more than a century. However, formal theories in the two domains have tended to look very different. This article argues that both faculties share the formal character of applicative systems based on operations corresponding to the same two combinatory operations, namely functional composition and type-raising. Viewing them in this way suggests simpler and more cognitively plausible accounts of both systems, and suggests that the language faculty evolved in the species and develops in children by a rather direct adaptation of a more primitive apparatus for planning purposive action in the world by composing affordances of objects or tools. The knowledge representation that underlies such planning is also reflected in the natural language semantics of tense, mood, and aspect, which the paper begins by arguing provides the key to understanding both systems.</abstract><cop>Heidelberg</cop><pub>Kluwer Academic Publishers</pub><doi>10.1023/a:1020820000972</doi><tpages>31</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0165-0157 |
ispartof | Linguistics and philosophy, 2002-12, Vol.25 (5/6), p.723-753 |
issn | 0165-0157 1573-0549 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85569647 |
source | International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS); JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection; Social Science Premium Collection; Springer Nature; Linguistics Collection; ProQuest One Literature; Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
subjects | Artificial intelligence Children Grammar Knowledge Language Linguistic theory Linguistics Mathematical functions Music composition Musical grammar Musical notation Natural language Ontology Philosophy Philosophy of language. Logic Semantics Sensory perception Tense Verbs Word order |
title | Plans, Affordances, and Combinatory Grammar |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-15T13%3A18%3A39IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Plans,%20Affordances,%20and%20Combinatory%20Grammar&rft.jtitle=Linguistics%20and%20philosophy&rft.au=Steedman,%20Mark&rft.date=2002-12-01&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=5/6&rft.spage=723&rft.epage=753&rft.pages=723-753&rft.issn=0165-0157&rft.eissn=1573-0549&rft.coden=LIPHD6&rft_id=info:doi/10.1023/a:1020820000972&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E25001872%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a405t-a4a0c69e8e4cf53189a2b011e3caffeb67e6fd28a2209c62f62684baf18aa2593%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=883708823&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_jstor_id=25001872&rfr_iscdi=true |