Loading…

Large analysis of genetic manipulations reveals an inverse correlation between initial alcohol resistance and rapid tolerance phenotypes

Tolerance occurs when, following an initial experience with a substance, more of the substance is required subsequently to induce identical behavioral effects. Tolerance is not well‐understood, and numerous researchers have turned to model organisms, particularly Drosophila melanogaster, to unravel...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Genes, brain and behavior brain and behavior, 2024-02, Vol.23 (1), p.e12884-n/a
Main Authors: Chvilicek, Maggie M., Seguin, Alexandra, Lathen, Daniel R., Titos, Iris, Cummins‐Beebee, Pearl N., Pabon, Miguel A., Miščević, Maša, Nickel, Emily, Merrill, Collin B., Rodan, Aylin R., Rothenfluh, Adrian
Format: Article
Language:English
Subjects:
Citations: Items that this one cites
Items that cite this one
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
Description
Summary:Tolerance occurs when, following an initial experience with a substance, more of the substance is required subsequently to induce identical behavioral effects. Tolerance is not well‐understood, and numerous researchers have turned to model organisms, particularly Drosophila melanogaster, to unravel its mechanisms. Flies have high translational relevance for human alcohol responses, and there is substantial overlap in disease‐causing genes between flies and humans, including those associated with Alcohol Use Disorder. Numerous Drosophila tolerance mutants have been described; however, approaches used to identify and characterize these mutants have varied across time and labs and have mostly disregarded any impact of initial resistance/sensitivity to ethanol on subsequent tolerance development. Here, we analyzed our own, as well as data published by other labs to uncover an inverse correlation between initial ethanol resistance and tolerance phenotypes. This inverse correlation suggests that initial resistance phenotypes can explain many ‘perceived’ tolerance phenotypes, thus classifying such mutants as ‘secondary’ tolerance mutants. Additionally, we show that tolerance should be measured as a relative increase in time to sedation between an initial and second exposure rather than an absolute change in time to sedation. Finally, based on our analysis, we provide a method for using a linear regression equation to assess the residuals of potential tolerance mutants. These residuals provide predictive insight into the likelihood of a mutant being a ‘primary’ tolerance mutant, where a tolerance phenotype is not solely a consequence of initial resistance, and we offer a framework for understanding the relationship between initial resistance and tolerance. Flies exposed to ethanol, and allowed to recover, develop tolerance, where fewer flies are sedated with the same exposure. Flies showing initial resistance to sedation, develop less tolerance after an identical first exposure, while flies initially sensitive, develop more tolerance.
ISSN:1601-1848
1601-183X
1601-183X
DOI:10.1111/gbb.12884