Loading…
Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears
Prey usually adjust anti-predator behavior to subtle variations in perceived risk. However, it is not clear whether adult large carnivores that are virtually free of natural predation adjust their behavior to subtle variations in human-derived risk, even when living in human-dominated landscapes. As...
Saved in:
Published in: | Oecologia 2011-05, Vol.166 (1), p.59-67 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Citations: | Items that this one cites Items that cite this one |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
cited_by | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3 |
---|---|
cites | cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3 |
container_end_page | 67 |
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 59 |
container_title | Oecologia |
container_volume | 166 |
creator | Ordiz, Andrés Støen, Ole-Gunnar Delibes, Miguel Swenson, Jon E. |
description | Prey usually adjust anti-predator behavior to subtle variations in perceived risk. However, it is not clear whether adult large carnivores that are virtually free of natural predation adjust their behavior to subtle variations in human-derived risk, even when living in human-dominated landscapes. As a model, we studied resting-site selection by a large carnivore, the brown bear (Ursus arctos), under different spatial and temporal levels of human activity. We quantified horizontal and canopy cover at 440 bear beds and 439 random sites at different distances from human settlements, seasons, and times of the day. We hypothesized that beds would be more concealed than random sites and that beds would be more concealed in relation to human-derived risk. Although human densities in Scandinavia are the lowest within bear ranges in Western Europe, we found an effect of human activity; bears chose beds with higher horizontal and canopy cover during the day (0700–1900 hours), especially when resting closer to human settlements, than at night (2200–0600 hours). In summer/fall (the berry season), with more intensive and dispersed human activity, including hunting, bears rested further from human settlements during the day than in spring (pre-berry season). Additionally, day beds in the summer/fall were the most concealed. Large carnivores often avoid humans at a landscape scale, but total avoidance in human-dominated areas is not possible. Apparently, bears adjust their behavior to avoid human encounters, which resembles the way prey avoid their predators. Bears responded to fine-scale variations in human-derived risk, both on a seasonal and a daily basis. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00442-011-1920-5 |
format | article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_swepu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_swepub_primary_oai_slubar_slu_se_58591</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A355308054</galeid><jstor_id>41499809</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>A355308054</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkk1v1DAQhiMEotvCD-AAikAIcUjxZ2KfUFXxUakSiAJXy7Eni5ckDnbSsv8eR1m2WoRAPow8ft7xjP1m2SOMTjFC1auIEGOkQBgXWBJU8DvZCjNK0o7Ku9kKISILwZk8yo5j3CCEGeb8fnZEMJGCsWqVff0YwOrRh5j7kA8Btq_zq0GPzhcjdIMPus2tiya4zvVzus99k3-bOt0XFoK7BpsHF7_n9Tavg7_p8xp0iA-ye41uIzzcxZPsy9s3n8_fF5cf3l2cn10WpuRyLEpJbGqlIhSRGlcVw8aCtLUVlUR1JaTRVmhWM2q0NhWxDZQccCkIp1wyS0-yYqkbb2CYajWkPnXYKq-diu1U6zAHFUFxwSVO_IuFH4L_MUEcVZeGg7bVPfgpKokILVkp_k-KElcSV6VI5NM_yI2fQp_GniEqJeJlgp4t0Fq3oFzf-DFoM5dUZ5RzigTiLFGnf6HSstA543toXMofCF4eCBIzws9xracY1cXVp0MWL6wJPsYAzf61MFKzn9TiJ5X8pGY_KZ40T3azTXUHdq_4baAEPN8BOhrdNkH3xsVbjmHMkUSJI7uPSkf9GsLtI_3r9seLaBOTQ_dFGWZSCiTpLzVx6P8</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>861399056</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears</title><source>JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection</source><source>Springer Nature</source><creator>Ordiz, Andrés ; Støen, Ole-Gunnar ; Delibes, Miguel ; Swenson, Jon E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Ordiz, Andrés ; Støen, Ole-Gunnar ; Delibes, Miguel ; Swenson, Jon E. ; Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet</creatorcontrib><description>Prey usually adjust anti-predator behavior to subtle variations in perceived risk. However, it is not clear whether adult large carnivores that are virtually free of natural predation adjust their behavior to subtle variations in human-derived risk, even when living in human-dominated landscapes. As a model, we studied resting-site selection by a large carnivore, the brown bear (Ursus arctos), under different spatial and temporal levels of human activity. We quantified horizontal and canopy cover at 440 bear beds and 439 random sites at different distances from human settlements, seasons, and times of the day. We hypothesized that beds would be more concealed than random sites and that beds would be more concealed in relation to human-derived risk. Although human densities in Scandinavia are the lowest within bear ranges in Western Europe, we found an effect of human activity; bears chose beds with higher horizontal and canopy cover during the day (0700–1900 hours), especially when resting closer to human settlements, than at night (2200–0600 hours). In summer/fall (the berry season), with more intensive and dispersed human activity, including hunting, bears rested further from human settlements during the day than in spring (pre-berry season). Additionally, day beds in the summer/fall were the most concealed. Large carnivores often avoid humans at a landscape scale, but total avoidance in human-dominated areas is not possible. Apparently, bears adjust their behavior to avoid human encounters, which resembles the way prey avoid their predators. Bears responded to fine-scale variations in human-derived risk, both on a seasonal and a daily basis.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0029-8549</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1432-1939</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1939</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00442-011-1920-5</identifier><identifier>PMID: 21298447</identifier><identifier>CODEN: OECOBX</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer</publisher><subject>Animal and plant ecology ; Animal, plant and microbial ecology ; Animals ; Anti-predator behavior ; Bears ; Beds ; Behavior, Animal ; BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY ; Behavioral ecology - Original Paper ; Biological and medical sciences ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Brown bears ; Canopies ; Carnivores ; Circadian Rhythm ; Ecology ; Environmental Sciences related to Agriculture and Land-use ; Female ; Fruits ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; General aspects ; Habitat conservation ; Habitat selection ; Human factors ; Human settlements ; Humans ; Hunting ; Hunting seasons ; Hydrology/Water Resources ; Landscape ; Life Sciences ; Male ; Mammalia ; Miljö- och naturvårdsvetenskap ; Plant Sciences ; Predation ; Predators ; Prey ; Seasons ; Site selection ; Summer ; Sweden ; Ursidae - psychology ; Ursus arctos ; Vertebrates: general zoology, morphology, phylogeny, systematics, cytogenetics, geographical distribution</subject><ispartof>Oecologia, 2011-05, Vol.166 (1), p.59-67</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag 2011</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2011 Springer</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/41499809$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/41499809$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925,58238,58471</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=24115090$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21298447$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://res.slu.se/id/publ/58591$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Ordiz, Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Støen, Ole-Gunnar</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Delibes, Miguel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Swenson, Jon E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet</creatorcontrib><title>Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears</title><title>Oecologia</title><addtitle>Oecologia</addtitle><addtitle>Oecologia</addtitle><description>Prey usually adjust anti-predator behavior to subtle variations in perceived risk. However, it is not clear whether adult large carnivores that are virtually free of natural predation adjust their behavior to subtle variations in human-derived risk, even when living in human-dominated landscapes. As a model, we studied resting-site selection by a large carnivore, the brown bear (Ursus arctos), under different spatial and temporal levels of human activity. We quantified horizontal and canopy cover at 440 bear beds and 439 random sites at different distances from human settlements, seasons, and times of the day. We hypothesized that beds would be more concealed than random sites and that beds would be more concealed in relation to human-derived risk. Although human densities in Scandinavia are the lowest within bear ranges in Western Europe, we found an effect of human activity; bears chose beds with higher horizontal and canopy cover during the day (0700–1900 hours), especially when resting closer to human settlements, than at night (2200–0600 hours). In summer/fall (the berry season), with more intensive and dispersed human activity, including hunting, bears rested further from human settlements during the day than in spring (pre-berry season). Additionally, day beds in the summer/fall were the most concealed. Large carnivores often avoid humans at a landscape scale, but total avoidance in human-dominated areas is not possible. Apparently, bears adjust their behavior to avoid human encounters, which resembles the way prey avoid their predators. Bears responded to fine-scale variations in human-derived risk, both on a seasonal and a daily basis.</description><subject>Animal and plant ecology</subject><subject>Animal, plant and microbial ecology</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anti-predator behavior</subject><subject>Bears</subject><subject>Beds</subject><subject>Behavior, Animal</subject><subject>BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY</subject><subject>Behavioral ecology - Original Paper</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Brown bears</subject><subject>Canopies</subject><subject>Carnivores</subject><subject>Circadian Rhythm</subject><subject>Ecology</subject><subject>Environmental Sciences related to Agriculture and Land-use</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fruits</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>General aspects</subject><subject>Habitat conservation</subject><subject>Habitat selection</subject><subject>Human factors</subject><subject>Human settlements</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hunting</subject><subject>Hunting seasons</subject><subject>Hydrology/Water Resources</subject><subject>Landscape</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mammalia</subject><subject>Miljö- och naturvårdsvetenskap</subject><subject>Plant Sciences</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Prey</subject><subject>Seasons</subject><subject>Site selection</subject><subject>Summer</subject><subject>Sweden</subject><subject>Ursidae - psychology</subject><subject>Ursus arctos</subject><subject>Vertebrates: general zoology, morphology, phylogeny, systematics, cytogenetics, geographical distribution</subject><issn>0029-8549</issn><issn>1432-1939</issn><issn>1432-1939</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2011</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNqFkk1v1DAQhiMEotvCD-AAikAIcUjxZ2KfUFXxUakSiAJXy7Eni5ckDnbSsv8eR1m2WoRAPow8ft7xjP1m2SOMTjFC1auIEGOkQBgXWBJU8DvZCjNK0o7Ku9kKISILwZk8yo5j3CCEGeb8fnZEMJGCsWqVff0YwOrRh5j7kA8Btq_zq0GPzhcjdIMPus2tiya4zvVzus99k3-bOt0XFoK7BpsHF7_n9Tavg7_p8xp0iA-ye41uIzzcxZPsy9s3n8_fF5cf3l2cn10WpuRyLEpJbGqlIhSRGlcVw8aCtLUVlUR1JaTRVmhWM2q0NhWxDZQccCkIp1wyS0-yYqkbb2CYajWkPnXYKq-diu1U6zAHFUFxwSVO_IuFH4L_MUEcVZeGg7bVPfgpKokILVkp_k-KElcSV6VI5NM_yI2fQp_GniEqJeJlgp4t0Fq3oFzf-DFoM5dUZ5RzigTiLFGnf6HSstA543toXMofCF4eCBIzws9xracY1cXVp0MWL6wJPsYAzf61MFKzn9TiJ5X8pGY_KZ40T3azTXUHdq_4baAEPN8BOhrdNkH3xsVbjmHMkUSJI7uPSkf9GsLtI_3r9seLaBOTQ_dFGWZSCiTpLzVx6P8</recordid><startdate>20110501</startdate><enddate>20110501</enddate><creator>Ordiz, Andrés</creator><creator>Støen, Ole-Gunnar</creator><creator>Delibes, Miguel</creator><creator>Swenson, Jon E.</creator><general>Springer</general><general>Springer-Verlag</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T7</scope><scope>7TN</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88A</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>BKSAR</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>L.G</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PCBAR</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7ST</scope><scope>7U6</scope><scope>ADTPV</scope><scope>AOWAS</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20110501</creationdate><title>Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears</title><author>Ordiz, Andrés ; Støen, Ole-Gunnar ; Delibes, Miguel ; Swenson, Jon E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2011</creationdate><topic>Animal and plant ecology</topic><topic>Animal, plant and microbial ecology</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anti-predator behavior</topic><topic>Bears</topic><topic>Beds</topic><topic>Behavior, Animal</topic><topic>BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY</topic><topic>Behavioral ecology - Original Paper</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Brown bears</topic><topic>Canopies</topic><topic>Carnivores</topic><topic>Circadian Rhythm</topic><topic>Ecology</topic><topic>Environmental Sciences related to Agriculture and Land-use</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fruits</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>General aspects</topic><topic>Habitat conservation</topic><topic>Habitat selection</topic><topic>Human factors</topic><topic>Human settlements</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hunting</topic><topic>Hunting seasons</topic><topic>Hydrology/Water Resources</topic><topic>Landscape</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mammalia</topic><topic>Miljö- och naturvårdsvetenskap</topic><topic>Plant Sciences</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Prey</topic><topic>Seasons</topic><topic>Site selection</topic><topic>Summer</topic><topic>Sweden</topic><topic>Ursidae - psychology</topic><topic>Ursus arctos</topic><topic>Vertebrates: general zoology, morphology, phylogeny, systematics, cytogenetics, geographical distribution</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ordiz, Andrés</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Støen, Ole-Gunnar</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Delibes, Miguel</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Swenson, Jon E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Industrial and Applied Microbiology Abstracts (Microbiology A)</collection><collection>Oceanic Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Biology Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>AUTh Library subscriptions: ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Earth, Atmospheric & Aquatic Science Collection</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences & Living Resources</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Journals</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Earth, Atmospheric & Aquatic Science Database</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Environment Abstracts</collection><collection>Sustainability Science Abstracts</collection><collection>SwePub</collection><collection>SwePub Articles</collection><jtitle>Oecologia</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ordiz, Andrés</au><au>Støen, Ole-Gunnar</au><au>Delibes, Miguel</au><au>Swenson, Jon E.</au><aucorp>Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet</aucorp><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears</atitle><jtitle>Oecologia</jtitle><stitle>Oecologia</stitle><addtitle>Oecologia</addtitle><date>2011-05-01</date><risdate>2011</risdate><volume>166</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>59</spage><epage>67</epage><pages>59-67</pages><issn>0029-8549</issn><issn>1432-1939</issn><eissn>1432-1939</eissn><coden>OECOBX</coden><abstract>Prey usually adjust anti-predator behavior to subtle variations in perceived risk. However, it is not clear whether adult large carnivores that are virtually free of natural predation adjust their behavior to subtle variations in human-derived risk, even when living in human-dominated landscapes. As a model, we studied resting-site selection by a large carnivore, the brown bear (Ursus arctos), under different spatial and temporal levels of human activity. We quantified horizontal and canopy cover at 440 bear beds and 439 random sites at different distances from human settlements, seasons, and times of the day. We hypothesized that beds would be more concealed than random sites and that beds would be more concealed in relation to human-derived risk. Although human densities in Scandinavia are the lowest within bear ranges in Western Europe, we found an effect of human activity; bears chose beds with higher horizontal and canopy cover during the day (0700–1900 hours), especially when resting closer to human settlements, than at night (2200–0600 hours). In summer/fall (the berry season), with more intensive and dispersed human activity, including hunting, bears rested further from human settlements during the day than in spring (pre-berry season). Additionally, day beds in the summer/fall were the most concealed. Large carnivores often avoid humans at a landscape scale, but total avoidance in human-dominated areas is not possible. Apparently, bears adjust their behavior to avoid human encounters, which resembles the way prey avoid their predators. Bears responded to fine-scale variations in human-derived risk, both on a seasonal and a daily basis.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer</pub><pmid>21298447</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00442-011-1920-5</doi><tpages>9</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0029-8549 |
ispartof | Oecologia, 2011-05, Vol.166 (1), p.59-67 |
issn | 0029-8549 1432-1939 1432-1939 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_swepub_primary_oai_slubar_slu_se_58591 |
source | JSTOR Archival Journals and Primary Sources Collection; Springer Nature |
subjects | Animal and plant ecology Animal, plant and microbial ecology Animals Anti-predator behavior Bears Beds Behavior, Animal BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY Behavioral ecology - Original Paper Biological and medical sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Brown bears Canopies Carnivores Circadian Rhythm Ecology Environmental Sciences related to Agriculture and Land-use Female Fruits Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology General aspects Habitat conservation Habitat selection Human factors Human settlements Humans Hunting Hunting seasons Hydrology/Water Resources Landscape Life Sciences Male Mammalia Miljö- och naturvårdsvetenskap Plant Sciences Predation Predators Prey Seasons Site selection Summer Sweden Ursidae - psychology Ursus arctos Vertebrates: general zoology, morphology, phylogeny, systematics, cytogenetics, geographical distribution |
title | Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears |
url | http://sfxeu10.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/loughborough?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-01T19%3A29%3A50IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_swepu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Predators%20or%20prey?%20Spatio-temporal%20discrimination%20of%20human-derived%20risk%20by%20brown%20bears&rft.jtitle=Oecologia&rft.au=Ordiz,%20Andr%C3%A9s&rft.aucorp=Sveriges%20lantbruksuniversitet&rft.date=2011-05-01&rft.volume=166&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=59&rft.epage=67&rft.pages=59-67&rft.issn=0029-8549&rft.eissn=1432-1939&rft.coden=OECOBX&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00442-011-1920-5&rft_dat=%3Cgale_swepu%3EA355308054%3C/gale_swepu%3E%3Cgrp_id%3Ecdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c659t-692d12972302b17741cde9dbd8790b789cad8a4b43caac72dfe65e168253594d3%3C/grp_id%3E%3Coa%3E%3C/oa%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=861399056&rft_id=info:pmid/21298447&rft_galeid=A355308054&rft_jstor_id=41499809&rfr_iscdi=true |